Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The coevolution of cooperation and dispersal in social groups and its implications for the emergence of multicellularity

Abstract

Background

Recent work on the complexity of life highlights the roles played by evolutionary forces at different levels of individuality. One of the central puzzles in explaining transitions in individuality for entities ranging from complex cells, to multicellular organisms and societies, is how different autonomous units relinquish control over their functions to others in the group. In addition to the necessity of reducing conflict over effecting specialized tasks, differentiating groups must control the exploitation of the commons, or else be out-competed by more fit groups.

Results

We propose that two forms of conflict – access to resources within groups and representation in germ line – may be resolved in tandem through individual and group-level selective effects. Specifically, we employ an optimization model to show the conditions under which different within-group social behaviors (cooperators producing a public good or cheaters exploiting the public good) may be selected to disperse, thereby not affecting the commons and functioning as germ line. We find that partial or complete dispersal specialization of cheaters is a general outcome. The propensity for cheaters to disperse is highest with intermediate benefit:cost ratios of cooperative acts and with high relatedness. An examination of a range of real biological systems tends to support our theory, although additional study is required to provide robust tests.

Conclusion

We suggest that trait linkage between dispersal and cheating should be operative regardless of whether groups ever achieve higher levels of individuality, because individual selection will always tend to increase exploitation, and stronger group structure will tend to increase overall cooperation through kin selected benefits. Cheater specialization as dispersers offers simultaneous solutions to the evolution of cooperation in social groups and the origin of specialization of germ and soma in multicellular organisms.

Background

Cooperation is central to transitions in individuality [1–4]. Full individuality is achieved when components cooperate and relinquish their autonomy to the larger whole. Depending on the type of transition, this may necessitate the division of labor in growth, reproduction, development, feeding, movement, and protection against external aggression and internal conflict [5, 6]. In the evolution of multicellularity, the chain of events from autonomous individuals at one level to the incorporation of these individuals into a more complex entity remains unclear [5]. However, some of the putative forces are likely to be general, since multicellularity has arisen many different times in evolutionary history [7, 8]. Moreover, that many groupings do not show sophisticated specialization and are characterized by substantial levels of internal conflict [9, 10], suggests that incomplete multicellularity may be a frequent outcome. What mechanisms are essential to generate individuality? We believe that a general theory needs to explain both full and incomplete transitions towards multicellular individuals.

Previous work highlights group and kin selection [5, 10, 11], organism size [12, 13], and the reorganization of fitness and specialization tradeoffs [14] as playing roles in the evolution of multicellularity. A feature common to these mechanisms is the establishment and maintenance of cooperative behaviors amongst subunits through, for example, conflict mediation (e.g. [15, 16]). Based on a recent literature review, Grosberg and Strathmann [8] argued that for cooperation to emerge and favor the specialization of subunits, groups of cells need to reduce genetic conflicts arising in cell lineages [10]. They conclude that several mechanisms can limit such conflicts, perhaps the most important being development from a single cell (e.g., [5, 16]).

A key type of subunit specialization in multicellular organisms is the separation of germ and soma [1, 5, 10, 17, 18]. Separating germ and somatic functions amongst individual cells or cell lineages requires that each sacrifice autonomy. Theory predicts that such specialization is promoted by non-mutually exclusive mechanisms such as cooperation and relatedness amongst cell lineages [10], cheater control [1, 19, 20] and adaptive responses to tradeoffs between survival and reproductive functions, i.e. a covariance effect augmenting the fitness of the group over the average fitness of its members [14]. It is not known whether the alignment of fitness interests in emerging soma and germ lines tends to occur before, during or after other types of specialization characteristic of multicellular organisms [12].

A pervasive feature in a diverse array of social systems is that individuals not contributing to the common good either act as dispersers, or are either rewarded for, or coerced into, cooperating. Examples range from bacteria (e.g. Pseudomonas fluorescens) through protozoa (e.g. Volvox carteri) to metazoans, like eusocial insects and mammals (see Additional file 1). For example, in naturally occurring Dictyostelium slime molds prespores secrete a chlorinated hexaphenone (DIF-1) inhibiting redifferentiation of prestalk cells into prespores, which would transpose them from "cooperative" stalk building to "cheating" spore production (i.e. a transition into the dispersing and perennial germ line; [21, 22]). Cheating is further curtailed by pleiotropic effects of a gene required to permit receipt of this signal, which affects also the probability of spore formation [23]. In tunicates such as Botryllus schlosseri, natural chimeras consisting of genetically nonhomogenous organisms often show reproducible germ cell parasitism that is sexually inherited, with "parasitic forms" being expressed only in the germ line, i.e. in the dispersing entities [24]. In the cooperatively breeding cichlid fish Neolamprologus pulcher, brood care helpers of both sexes are forced to pay rent for being tolerated in a safe territory [25, 26]. To avoid being punished they preemptively appease dominants by cooperative and submissive behavior [27]. Typically, in these cichlids and in cooparatively breeding meerkats Suricata suricatta, subordinates preparing for dispersal reduce helping [28, 29], which might be explained by reduced costs of potential punishment by eviction [30, 31]. In eusocial mole rats (Heterocephalus glaber and Cryptomys damarensis) non-reproductive helpers and hardly helping dispersers coexist [32–34]. Policing of subordinates by dominant breeders may simultaneously maintain social order and stimulate cooperative behaviors [35, 36]. This distinction of roles between individuals is particularly obvious in the separation between soma and germ that has apparently evolved many times independently [7]. Nevertheless, there are examples where cooperative behaviors are associated with enhanced group dispersal (see Additional file 1). For example, in the soil-dwelling social bacterium Myxococcus xanthus, individualistic cell movement ('A-motility') promotes swarming on hard surfaces, whereas swarming on soft surfaces is a group function driven primarily by individually costly S-motility [37].

These empirical patterns merit explanation, and we take a first step by employing optimization techniques to evaluate the conditions leading to associations between dispersal and social strategy. Sociality in our models takes the form of cooperation in the production of a public good. Previous study of public goods has shown how cheating, if left unchecked, potentially leads to a "tragedy of the commons" [38, 39], whereby individual selection tends to favor exploitation of the public good at some concurrent or future detriment of the group. Several non-mutually exclusive mechanisms may promote cooperation and group persistence, including kin selection (e.g., [40–42]), rewards and sanctions (e.g., [43, 44]), spatial and network structure (e.g., [45–47]), and signals involving kin or non-kin (e.g., [48–50]). Recent reviews and perspectives can be found in Crespi [51], Sachs and colleagues [52], Lehmann and Keller [53], and West and coworkers [54].

We develop a model based on kin selection that incorporates dispersal specialization, as suggested by the case studies in Table S1 (see Additional file 1). We employ the terms "soma" and "germ" to represent the functions of within-group growth and dispersal leading to the founding of new groups, respectively. Our use of the terms "cooperators" and "cheaters" refers to social behaviors within the commons (e.g., soma), and this should be distinguished from the frequent usage of "cheaters" as cooperative somatic lineages trying to gain access to germ line (e.g., [1, 5, 8, 10, 22]). Specifically, cooperators contribute to the public good within a distinct group at an individual cost, and cheaters exploit the public good. Cooperators and/or cheaters may be selected to either remain in a group, or to disperse (potentially founding new groups). Our theory proposes a mechanism leading to high overall cooperation, based on dispersal specialization. In addition to increasing our understanding of cooperative and dispersal behaviors, it could apply to the evolution of multicellularity in a range of contexts, including physiologically integrated organisms [55, 56], organisms with both solitary and integrated life-styles (e.g., [57]), and complex societies [58].

Methods

We formalize our verbal arguments given above by developing and analyzing a model of coevolution between exploitation of the commons and dispersal. From the outset, we stress that our model is a highly simplified representation of this process, and not aimed to make quantitative predictions for any given system. Rather, our goal is to identify the qualitatively important drivers in the coevolution of individual strategies and the evolution of multicellularity.

In our model the focal units of selection are individuals themselves, rather than the higher-level unit. A transition to multicellularity is favored when the interests of the individual and the higher-level (the group) are aligned [5, 8, 15]. Previous models investigating the transition to multicellularity invoke a framework where the group is the focal unit of selection (see, for example [15]). However, focusing on the higher-level as the focal unit does not easily allow the investigation of optimization at the lower level [59], and the individually-selected conditions leading to a major transition [60]. Grosberg and Strathmann [8] have argued that many of the requirements for transitions to multicellularity exist in unicellular organisms (for social groups, see [61]). Once a transition is in progress, and the "group" begins to behave as an individual entity, one can begin to treat this unit as an evolving individual in itself.

We analyze an optimization model that takes into account the effect of both the phenotype of the focal individual and the average phenotype of the group in which it lives, on the fitness of the focal individual (see Table 1 for descriptions of parameters and variables). The approach is based on the direct fitness method [42, 62] in that, by considering the effects of both individual and average group phenotypes on the fitness of a focal individual, we can apply the Price Equation to partition these effects as weighted by the relatedness of the focal individual to other members of the group [42]. We can then assess the relative impacts of (1) costs and benefits of individual behaviors and (2) kin structure, on associations between exploitative strategy within a group, and dispersal to found new groups. Nevertheless, our model oversimplifies the complexity of social behavior and dispersal decisions (for review, see [63]), and should thus be viewed as a preliminary attempt to identify patterns.

Table 1 Parameters and variables used in this study.

Our model makes several assumptions. First, we do not explicitly consider dynamics, such as group founding, group numbers, individual emigration and immigration, and competition for limiting resources within or between groups. Rather, we assume negligible variation in inter-group competition. Second, our model does not explicitly incorporate genetic polymorphisms, meaning that the heritable traits are probabilities to adopt alternatives of each strategy (disperse or stay; cooperate or cheat) depending on environmental and/or social conditions [1, 10, 32, 64–66]. Third, there is a simple direct tradeoff between an individual's viability (growth, survival and reproduction) within the group and its ability to disperse and found new groups. This is based on the well established life-history trade-off between reproduction and dispersal (see [67]), probably best studied in insects (on the physiological scale e.g. [68–70]; on the ecological scale e.g. [71, 72]). Whereas growth and reproduction within the group impacts the production and consumption of the public good, the tendency to disperse reduces these impacts because of the limited presence of dispersers in the source group.

Life cycle and fitness equations

We assume that a group's life-cycle has three sequential stages: colonization, growth, reproduction and survival of individuals within the group; exhaustion of resources; and the dispersal of survivors. Some of the survivors may stay at the same site of the source group, and others disperse as colonists to other sites.

The model tracks the fitness contribution of a mutant individual i, within group j [42, 62]. Fitness effects are partitioned between cooperators and cheats–who have positive and negative impacts on the public good, respectively–and amongst dispersal strategies. Thus four strategies are possible: (1) cooperate and remain in group, (2) cooperate and disperse, (3) cheat and remain in group, and (4) cheat and disperse. Only the first and third strategies affect the public good.

The proportion of cooperators in the group is n i (for simplicity, hereafter we denote individual i within group j using the subscript i only), which can take continuous values between 0 and 1. Moreover, our model incorporates two dispersal strategies based on whether the dispersing individual is a cooperator or a cheater. We define y i as the investment of a cooperator in dispersal and z i as the investment of a given cheater in dispersal. Both of these quantities take on continuous values between zero and one. The mean proportions of dispersing cooperators and cheaters in group j are y j n j and z j (1-n j ), respectively and overall investment in dispersal is d j = y j n j + z j (1-n j ).

The fitness equation takes the form

w i = D(n i , y i , z i ) E(n i , y i , z i ) G(n i , y i , z i ), (1)

where the functions D and E, respectively, represent the contribution of selection on dispersal and the exploitation of the public good of individual i in group j to its own fitness. Function G is the overall investment in the public good in group j.

Dispersal is modeled by considering the fitness contributions of both individuals that stay at the site previously occupied by the group and others that disperse [73]. We assume that the costs of dispersal may differ between cooperators (c) and cheaters (e). Small costs would indicate abundant new sites for group establishment and high disperser survival. Although we consider different cases in the analysis, our general expectation is that the costs of cooperation will extend to dispersal, such that c > e.

The function, D, takes the form

D(n i , y i , z i ) = [(1 - z i (1-n i ) - y i n i )/(1 - z j (1-n j ) - y j n j + (1-e) z (1-n) + (1-c)y n)] + [((1-e) z i (1-n i ) + (1-c)y i n i )/(1 - e z (1-n) - c y n)]. (2)

The first term in square brackets describes the fitness of a non-disperser (1 - z i (1-n i ) - y i n i ) relative to the average non-disperser (1 - z j (1-n j ) - y j n j ) and immigrants ((1-e) z (1-n) + (1-c)y n). The second term describes the fitness of a disperser ((1-e) z i (1-n i ) + (1-c)y i n i ) given the competition it faces with residents (1 - z (1-n) - y n) and migrants ((1-e) z (1-n) + (1-c)y n) in another group. The terms n, z and y (i.e., without subscripts) are population-wide means. The denominator in both terms represents the amount of competition faced either in the original group, in the case of a non-disperser, or in a new group, in the case of the disperser. Note that in the limit of no dispersal, individual fitness can still be positive under the assumption that groups survive indefinitely.

All non-dispersing individuals are selected to exploit, but given our assumption that there is a cost of cooperation (s), this will weight selection to favoring cheaters, all else being equal. The function, E, describes the contribution of individual i to its own fitness through exploitation of the public good and is given by

E(n i , y i , z i ) = [(1-z i ) (1-n i ) + (1-s) (1-y i ) n i ]/[(1-z j ) (1-n j ) + (1-s) (1-y j ) n j ], (3)

where the subscript j indicates mean group levels, and the constant s measures the cost to individual cooperators in producing the public good.

The overall effect of group investment in the public good on individual fitness is described by

G(n i , y i , z i ) = 1 + P (1-y j ) n j - Q (1-z j ) (1-n j ), (4)

where it is assumed that non-dispersing cooperators have a positive effect on the public good (scaled by P) as their frequency, n j , increases [74, 75], whereas cheaters have a net negative effect on the public good (scaled by Q) as their frequency, 1-n j , increases. Note that in the absence of cooperators, cheats can persist as long as their impact on the commons is sufficiently low (z Q< 1). Alternatively, when group effects are nil (i.e. P = Q = 0), the notion of a group is a collection of autonomous individuals.

Relatedness and numerical simulation methods

We analyze the model by employing the Price Equation, which enables us to express possible fitness maxima as a function of constant parameters and variables, and the relatedness, r, between individuals. Taylor and Frank [62] give methods for finding the equilibrium, such that for any trait v we have

dw i /dv i = ∂w i /∂v i + r ∂w i /∂v j (5)

from which we can find a steady state(s) when dw i /dv i = 0 to find any or all v* = y*, z*, n*.

In our model, r can either be a parameter (referred to as an "open model" by Gardner and West [76]) or can emerge from the underlying structure of the population (referred to as a "closed" model in [76]). In the latter case, we may derive r from the dispersal of individuals in the population with the recursion relation (e.g., [39, 76])

r(t+1) = 1/k + (k - 1)/k (1 - d)2 r(t). (6)

This recursion tracks the probability that a given focal individual is identical by descent to another randomly picked individual at time t. The parameter k is the effective number of individuals in the group, and can be viewed as a measure of genetic diversity due to individual aggregation in group founding and habitat structure. [Note however that our model does not explicitly track the actual number of individuals in the group]. Low k is indicative of group founding by single individuals, group resistance to immigration, and abundant open sites for group founding [10, 77].

In the recursion above, the term 1/k represents the probability that the randomly picked individual is the focal individual itself. The second term represents the probability that the randomly picked individual is different to the focal individual, and that neither have dispersed (represented by (1-d)2). This is multiplied by the relatedness from the previous round. Solving this recursion relation yields the equilibrium relatedness, which is

r = 1/(k - (k - 1) (1 - d)2). (7)

As we assume weak selection, the probability that a given individual disperses depends on the probability that it is a cooperator and disperses, plus the probability that it is a cheater and disperses, so d = yn+z(1-n) in this case. Under the assumptions of weak selection, we evaluate this recursion for the case when v i = v j = v, where v is the trait in question.

Optimal strategies were solved numerically. This consisted of iterating equation (5) with steps of 0.05 or smaller for a total of 100,000 steps, which was sufficient to identify the steady state in all cases. We found that whereas initial levels of evolving variables did not affect the optimal solution when only dispersal frequencies y and z evolved, initial conditions could indeed affect the optimal solution when all three variables evolved. Closer examination showed that alternative stable states were possible, one with either all cheaters (n* = 0) or all cooperators (n* = 1), and a second with both strategies persisting (0 <n* < 1). Although we cannot exclude the existence of alternative interior equilibria, our numerical studies always yielded at most a single interior solution.

Results

We consider two scenarios. In the first (Model 1) only dispersal in cooperators (y) and cheaters (z) evolves, but not cooperation (n). This situation would be obtained if mechanisms not explicitly included in the model (e.g., policing, [44]) controlled the level of cooperation, or if the frequencies of cooperative behaviors were either not subject to evolution, or labile to it over much longer time scales than dispersal. More generally however, empirical study suggests that cooperative behaviors are subject to selection [78–80] and we consider the case (Model 2) in which dispersal and the frequency of cooperators (n) and cheaters (1-n) co-evolve.

In addition to optimal levels of dispersal (Model 1), and of cooperation and dispersal (Model 2), we examine the effects of model parameters on dispersal specialization σ = y*/(y*+z*), and for Model 2 only, overall cooperation Φ = n*(1-y*)+(1-n*)z* (i.e., the sum of cooperators not dispersing and of cheaters dispersing). Note that when σ = 0 (or σ = 1), although all cooperators (cheats for σ = 1) are sedentary it is not necessarily true that all cheats (cooperators for σ = 1) disperse.

Model 1

Optimal solutions always yielded partial or complete specialization, with cooperators tending to disperse more than cheaters (i.e., σ > 0.5) for high costs of cooperation (s) compared to public good's effect (P), and low cooperator frequencies (n) (Figure 1). The reverse trends promote relative cheater dispersal (σ < 0.5; Fig. 1). The impact of effective group size (k) is more complex. Higher k tends to polarize dispersal to either cooperators (y* > 0, z* = 0) or cheaters (y* = 0, z* > 0), and increases the parameter space in which cooperators dominate dispersal (areas with σ * = 1; Fig. 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Globally optimal associations in dispersal and exploitation strategy for Model 1. Axes: P measures the impact of the public good on individual fitness, and s is the individual cost to cooperators in contributing to the public good. σ * = y*/(z* + y*) indexes the tendency of cooperators to disperse (σ* > 0.5) or cheats to disperse (σ* < 0.5). Thick curves demarcate areas of parameter space yielding different levels of σ, whereas thin lines show areas in with either y* = 1 or z* = 1. Caption a: k = 1.2, n = 0.1; caption b: k = 10, n = 0.1; caption c: k = 1.2, n = 0.9; caption d: k = 10, n = 0.9. Note that for legibility, very thin areas parallel to thick lines are omitted, in which 0.5 < σ* < 1 for caption c, and 0 < σ* < 1 for caption d. Unless otherwise noted, dispersal rates are greater than zero and less than unity. Other parameters: c = e = 0.2, Q = 0.2. See main text for numerical methods.

Low effective group size (low k) should positively associate with kin competition, and in agreement with previous work [81, 82], we find that low k is associated with higher overall dispersal, d* (Figure 2a). Not surprisingly, d* increases with lower cooperator frequencies (n) and public good effects (P) (Fig. 2a). However, the effects of k and n on the separate cooperator (y*) and cheater (z*) dispersal frequencies are more complex (Figs. 2b, c). In particular, low k was always found to drive cheaters to disperse (Fig. 2c), whereas the effect on cooperators depended strongly on cooperator frequency (n) and public good productivity (P) (Fig. 2b).

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of parameters on optimal dispersal levels for Model 1. Effects of public good production (P), frequency of cooperators (n) and effective group size (k). Caption a: overall dispersal d*; Caption b: investment in cooperator dispersal y*; Caption c investment in cheater dispersal z*. Thin line: k = 1.2, n = 0.1; dashed line: k = 10, n = 0.1; thick line: k = 1.2, n = 0.9; thick dashed line: k = 10, n = 0.9. Other parameters: c = e = 0.2, Q = 0.2, s = 0.6.

Cheater and cooperator dispersal can be understood as follows. When the group is dominated by cheaters (low n) and production of the public good (P) is small, increasing cooperator sedentariness (1-y*) has little beneficial effect on fitness (w), due to insufficient marginal gains via both individual exploitation (E; eqn. 3) and the group effects (G; eqn. 4). As a consequence, cooperators are selected to disperse more, relative to cheaters. Cheaters may disperse at high levels nonetheless (e.g., case of n = 0.1, k = 1.2 in Fig. 2c), because in so doing, they lessen the effects of the tragedy of the commons on individual fitness of their kin. In contrast, when the group is dominated by cooperators (high n) and public good production is high (P), marginal fitness increases with cooperator sedentariness and, due to kin competition (k), cheaters are selected to disperse more, relative to cooperators.

Model 2

Permitting social evolution introduces the possibility that the frequency of cooperators or cheaters fixes to zero or one, in which case associations (σ) between dispersal and social strategies are irrelevant. We find that depending on parameter combinations, either only a single global optimum is obtained, or two alternative local optima are possible. In the latter case, which state is obtained depends on initial levels of y, z and n in the numerical simulations. Figure 3 shows the fraction of simulations with random initial levels of n, y and z, achieving either an internal equilibrium (0 <n* < 1), or one with all cooperators (n* = 1), or one with all cheaters (n* = 0) for different costs of cooperator dispersal (c; Fig. 3a) and effective group sizes (k, Fig. 3b). For simplicity in the analyses below, we employ a single arbitrary starting condition (n = y = z = 0.5).

Figure 3
figure 3

The fraction of simulations in Model 2 leading to different local optima. Results based on 100 simulations in which initial levels of n, y, and z are each set to a random number between zero and one, inclusive. These simulations produced one of three equilibria: n* = 0, 0 <n* < 1 or n* = 1. Caption a effect of the cost of cooperator dispersal (c) with P = Q = 0.3, s = 0.5, k = 2, e = 0.2; caption b effect of effective group size (k) with P = Q = 0.2, s = 0.6, e = 0.2, c = 0.3.

We observed four basic outcomes (Fig. 4): (1) fixation of cooperators (n* = 1), (2) fixation of cheaters (n* = 0), or coexistence of cooperators and cheaters with (3) the former only being sedentary (σ * = 0), or (4) the latter only being sedentary (σ * = 1). When σ * = 0 or σ * = 1 (i.e., all cooperators or cheaters sedentary, respectively), we further found outcomes in which all cheaters dispersed (z* = 1) or all cooperators dispersed (y* = 1), respectively. Parameter effects are generally similar to Model 1, but with some notable contrasts.

Figure 4
figure 4

Locally optimal associations between dispersal and exploitation strategy. The frequency of dispersal in cooperators (y) and cheaters (z) evolves, and the frequency of cooperators (n) and cheaters (1-n) evolves. Initial frequencies in numerical studies: y = z = n = 0.5. As for Figure 1 except caption a: k = 1.2, c = 0.1; caption b: k = 10, c = 0.1; caption c: k = 1.2, c = 0.3; caption d: k = 10, c = 0.3.

Whereas in Model 1, the relative cost of cooperator (c) and cheater (e) dispersal did not yield a simple threshold condition for optimal outcomes (not shown), it did so for Model 2. We found that when cooperators and cheaters coexisted and e > c, cooperators dispersed and cheaters did not (i.e., σ * = 1) (Figs. 4a, b). The reverse held when c > e (Figs. 4c, d). Low effective group size (k) increases cooperator persistence (i.e., smaller areas in which n* = 0 in Fig. 4), with the effects on cheater persistence contingent on other parameters (i.e., differences in areas with n* = 0 in Fig. 4). More interestingly, whereas when e > c, lower k shifts the parameter space permitting cooperators and cheaters to coexist and has little effect on the area in which all cooperators disperse (y* = 1), when c > e, it expands the area of coexistence and that in which all cheaters disperse (z* = 1) (Fig. 4). Finally, relatedness (r*) generally increases with high P:s ratios, low k, and high costs to cooperator dispersal, c, with respect to cheater dispersal, e (Fig. 5). Interestingly, specialization in dispersal by cheaters and in sedentariness by cooperators tends to associate with high, but not the highest levels of relatedness (cf Figs. 4c, 5c).

Figure 5
figure 5

Relatedness, r* , associated with simulations in Figure 4.

If we define the functional role of a cooperator as contributing to the public good, and that well functioning groups minimize the impact of cheats on the public good, then, trivially, specialization resulting in mobile cooperators and sedentary cheaters corresponds to a non-social, individualistic scenario, and cannot be considered a group related phenomenon. There are however two ways in which the impact of cheaters on the commons can be reduced: either 1-n* decreases and/or z* increases. Figure 6 presents the effects of model processes on overall cooperation, defined as Φ = n* (1-y*) + (1-n*) z*. We see that although high levels of Φ are generally promoted for high P:s ratios, perfect overall cooperation (Φ = 1) is most readily obtained at low k and intermediate P:s ratios (e.g. Fig. 6c).

Figure 6
figure 6

Overall cooperation, Φ = n *(1 - y *)+(1- n *) z *, associated with simulations in Figure 4.

Discussion

Our results are in broad agreement with the tenets of kin selection theory for explaining dispersal [45, 81, 82] and the maintenance of cooperative behaviors [83–85]. Specifically, we found that dispersal specialization leading to high levels of overall cooperation (Φ) is promoted by sufficient benefit to cost ratios (P : s) of cooperation and by kin selection (low k). The one apparent discrepancy to previous theory is, whereas higher benefit (P) to cost (s) ratios promote cooperation, higher kin selection (low k) was sometimes observed to reduce the relative frequency of cooperators (n*) (cf. Figs. 4c, d). This can be explained if we consider cheaters dispersing from the group as a type of cooperative behavior. Dispersing cheaters are effectively 'cooperative' because of the incurred individual cost of dispersal (e), and the benefits to the group in having less negative impact on the commons (Q) (cf. Figs 4c, d with Figs. 6c, d). Moreover, we found that partial or total specialization of otherwise somatic cheats as dispersing germ line occurred without the need for costly modifiers [86] or the repression of cheaters [[87, 88], but see [22]], suggesting that the mechanism identified here is applicable to a wider range of organisms where these mechanisms do not sufficiently reduce somatic cheating, or cannot evolve. Conversely, control mechanisms such as rewarding and punishment, which might be operating in many systems (see examples in Additional file 1), do not preclude the functionality of the mechanism demonstrated in this study (cf. Model 1).

The examples presented in Table S1 (see Additional file 1) and our theoretical findings suggest a common conceptual and mechanistic foundation for the evolution of cooperation and individual functional specialization within groups (e.g., multicellularity). Most of the empirical examples share the feature that cooperators are less dispersive than more competitive individuals. For instance, low dispersal coincides with physical binding in bacteria that generate biofilms as a public good by polymer production [79, 89] (but see ref. [90] for an alternative interpretation), with alloparental care of offspring in cooperative breeding [91], or with complete genetic altruism in certain eusocial insects [92]. It is worth noting that a consistent differentiation of roles regarding sedentariness and dispersal in relation to cooperation and cheating may be much more common in nature than currently believed (e.g. [93]). Because there is no prior formal theory predicting such a relationship, empirical research on this issue is rare and suitable data are therefore scant. We stress that our theory does not elucidate the precise evolutionary pathway leading to complete multicellularity [16, 77], but rather assesses the forces promoting or forestalling different levels of specialization of cooperators and cheaters as functional germ line and soma. As such, the observations of biased dispersal in Table S1 (see Additional file 1) have alternative explanations, including forced eviction [94] and individual-based habitat selection [95]. Experimental (e.g., [79, 80, 96–98]), phylogenetic (e.g., [6]), and theoretical (e.g., [13, 22] and see discussion below) approaches are fruitful avenues to explore alternative explanations and pathways.

Transitions in individuality and social complexity are generally thought to require some form of reduction in genetic variance during the reproductive process [20, 77]. Genetic heterogeneity can emerge from many sources [99], and the recursive equation 6 in our framework greatly simplifies these, only explicitly including the effects of dispersal. Our results confirm the importance of relatedness in achieving multicellularity, but also show that the highest levels of relatedness did not necessarily yield full specialization of cooperators or cheaters as dispersers, and that complete specialization could occur at relatedness levels as low as 0.7 (Fig. 4d). As such, our findings could extend to some systems in which groups are formed by the initial aggregation of non-kin [10, 74, 87, 99]. Further study is needed to explore this prediction in detail, since our model did not explicitly account for different lineages, and as such we do not know how spatial heterogeneities in relatedness might influence our results [100].

Our findings have precedent, both in the study of symbiotic associations, and investigations of cooperation within species. With regard to host-parasite and symbiotic interactions, previous research has considered how parasite virulence (which is analogous to cheaters exploiting cooperative groups) may evolve spatially (e.g., [101]; for reviews see [102, 103]). In the case of horizontal transmission in parasites, which is analogous to the level of dispersal in our model (see also [73]), theory generally predicts that increased horizontal transmission (z in our model) associates with higher parasite virulence (Q (1-n) in our model) [103]. Despite allowing for relatedness between potential cooperators and cheats we have a comparable finding, whereby an increasing tragedy of the commons pushes cheating individuals to disperse; this is both because of increased individual fitness opportunities through dispersal (z) and increased inclusive fitness through lowered group effects for those related individuals that do not disperse (Q(1-n) (1-z)).

In a model investigating cooperation in spatially viscous environments, van Baalen and Rand [45] suggested that non-altruists should disperse more readily than altruists and hypothesized that this could be viewed as a transition towards multicellularity. Koella [104] studied the independent dispersal of altruists and of cheaters in a spatially explicit setting and found that a polymorphism could arise in which altruists dispersed and interacted locally, whereas cheaters evolved longer dispersal distances and exploited altruistic clusters. Hamilton and Taborsky [95] showed that when the propensities to cooperate by generalized reciprocity and to disperse evolve independently, under a wide range of conditions either cooperation or defection is associated with dispersal, depending on the probability of finding new groups and on the costs of being alone. Over most of the range of mobility costs examined, cooperation was negatively correlated with mobility, while defection was not. Ultimately, this leads to assortment between altruists and defectors in the population (see also [105]), which secondarily can generate group selection effects [106, 107]. Hamilton and Taborsky [95] did not check for linkage effects, however. In another study of the joint evolution of altruism and mobility, Le Galliard and coworkers [108] found that more altruism enhancing local aggregation can select for increased mobility. The synergistic selective interaction between altruism and mobility may cause dispersal to be considerably higher than that predicted in a purely selfish population, if altruism costs accelerate slowly and mobility costs are moderate. However, their model did not reveal a polymorphism to occur between selfish-mobile and altruistic-sessile phenotypes as found so often in nature, from microbes and unicellular algae to mammals (e.g. [18, 32, 109]; Additional file 1). Queller [10] argued that the resolution of within-organism conflicts could occur if an altruism allele is expressed conditional on the environment, the altruistic act being an individual removing itself from the germ line in order to perform an enhanced somatic activity. Rainey [110] verbally proposed an idea similar in some respects to these studies, in which group selection acts to promote the functional separation of germ and soma in bacterial biofilms through the dispersal of cheats (see also [79]). Finally, Michod [14] showed how the specialization of lower level units into germ and soma could be associated with the transfer of fitness from lower units to the new higher individual. A critical feature of his model is the tradeoff between the viability and fecundity of lower level units, which, for convex relationships, creates disruptive selection for cooperative germ and soma. Our study, whilst generating congruent results, is to our knowledge the first to demonstrate that the evolution of lower level units based on their effects on the commons can yield dispersal specialization, one of the precursors for selection at the group level and the evolution of full multicellularity.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that the establishment of trait linkage between dispersal and the propensity of within-group cheating may be a general phenomenon promoting complex social organization and multicellularity. Importantly, we cautiously suggest this should be operative regardless of whether groups ever achieve higher levels of individuality, because selection on individual components will always tend to increase exploitation, and stronger group structure will tend to increase overall cooperation through kin selected benefits [42, 84]. Partial or full reduction in the negative effects of cheaters on the commons through their specialization as dispersers offers partial solutions to two problems: the evolution of cooperation in social groups and the origin of the specialization of germ and soma in multicellular organisms. Our model is, nevertheless, a highly simplified caricature of real systems and future theoretical and empirical study is needed to explore its robustness.

References

  1. Buss LW: The Evolution of Individuality. 1987, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  2. Michod RE: Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of individuality. II. Conflict mediation. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1996, 263: 813-822. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0121.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Michod RE, Roze D: Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of individuality. III. Transitions in the unit of fitness. Mathematical and Computational Biology: Computational Morphogenesis, Hierarchical Complexity, and Digital Evolution. Edited by: Nehaniv CL. 1999, Providence: American Mathematical Society, 47-92.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Michod RE, Nedelcu AM, Roze D: Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of individuality IV. Conflict mediation and evolvability in Volvox carteri. Biosystems. 2003, 69: 95-114. 10.1016/S0303-2647(02)00133-8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Maynard Smith J, Szathmary E: The Major Transitions in Evolution. 1995, New York: WH Freeman

    Google Scholar 

  6. Bell G, Mooers AO: Size and complexity among multicellular organisms. Biol J Linn Soc. 1997, 60: 345-363. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1997.tb01500.x.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bonner JT: The Origins of Multicellularity. Integr Biol. 1998, 1: 27-36. 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6602(1998)1:1<27::AID-INBI4>3.0.CO;2-6.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Grosberg RK, Strathmann RR: The evolution of multicellularity: A minor major transition. Ann Rev Ecol Syst. 2007, 38: 621-654. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102403.114735.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Kirk DL: A twelve-step program for evolving multicellularity and a division of labor. Bioessays. 2005, 27: 299-310. 10.1002/bies.20197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Queller DC: Relatedness and the fraternal major transitions. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B. 2000, 355: 1647-1655. 10.1098/rstb.2000.0727.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Wilson DS: Theory of group selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1975, 72: 143-146. 10.1073/pnas.72.1.143.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bell G: The origin and early evolution of germ cells as illustrated in the Volvocales. The Origin and Evolution of Sex. Edited by: Halvorson H, Monroy A. 1985, New York: Liss, 221-56.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Pfeiffer T, Bonhoeffer S: An evolutionary scenario for the transition to undifferentiated multicellularity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003, 100: 1095-1098. 10.1073/pnas.0335420100.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Michod RE: The group covariance effect and fitness trade-offs during evolutionary transitions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006, 103: 9113-9117. 10.1073/pnas.0601080103.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Michod R: Darwinian Dynamics. 1999, Princeton; Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  16. Michod RE: Cooperation and conflict during the origin of multicellularity. Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. Edited by: Hammerstein P. 2003, Cambridge: MIT Press, 261-307.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Hurst LD: Parasite diversity and the evolution of diploidy, multicellularity and anisogamy. J theor Biol. 1990, 144: 429-443. 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80085-2.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Koufopanou V, Bell G: Soma and germ: an experimental approach using Volvox. Proc R Soc London Ser B. 1993, 254: 107-13. 10.1098/rspb.1993.0134.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Otto SP, Hastings IM: Mutation and selection within the individual. Genetica. 1998, 102/103: 507-24. 10.1023/A:1017074823337.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Frank SA: Host control of symbiont transmission: the separation of symbionts into germ and soma. Am Nat. 1996, 148: 1113-1124. 10.1086/285974.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Firtel RA: Integration of signaling information in controlling cell-fate decisions in Dictyostelium. Genes and Development. 1995, 9: 1427-1444. 10.1101/gad.9.12.1427.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Hudson RE, Aukema JE, Rispe C, Roze D: Altruism, cheating, and anitcheater adaptations in cellular slime molds. Am Nat. 2002, 160: 31-43. 10.1086/340613.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Foster KR, Shaulsky G, Strassmann JE, Queller DC, Thompson CRL: Pleiotropy as a mechanism to stabilize cooperation. Nature. 2004, 431: 693-696. 10.1038/nature02894.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Rinkevich B: Natural chimerism in colonial urochordates. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology. 2005, 322: 93-109. 10.1016/j.jembe.2005.02.020.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Taborsky M: Breeder-helper conflict in a cichlid fish with broodcare helpers – an experimental analysis. Behaviour. 1985, 95: 45-75. 10.1163/156853985X00046.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Balshine-Earn S, Neat FC, Reid H, Taborsky M: Paying to stay or paying to breed? Field evidence for direct benefits of helping behavior in a cooperatively breeding fish. Behavioral Ecology. 1998, 5: 432-438. 10.1093/beheco/9.5.432.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Bergmüller R, Taborsky M: Experimental manipulation of helping in a cooperative breeder: helpers 'pay to stay' by pre-emptive appeasement. Animal Behaviour. 2005, 69: 19-28. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.009.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Bergmüller R, Heg D, Taborsky M: Helpers in a cooperatively breeding cichlid stay and pay or disperse and breed, depending on ecological constraints. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2005, 272: 325-331. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2960.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Young AJ, Carlson AA, Clutton-Brock T: Trade-offs between extraterritorial prospecting and helping in a cooperative mammal. Animal Behaviour. 2005, 70: 829-837. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.019.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Skubic E, Taborsky M, McNamara JM, Houston AI: When to parasitize? A dynamic optimization model of reproductive strategies in a cooperative breeder. J theor Biol. 2004, 227: 487-501. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2003.11.021.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Stephens PA, Russell AF, Young AJ, Sutherland WJ, Clutton-Brock TH: Dispersal, eviction, and conflict in meerkats (Suricata suricatta): an evolutionarily stable strategy model. Am Nat. 2005, 165: 120-35. 10.1086/426597.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. O'Riain MJ, Jarvis JUM, Faulkes CG: A dispersive morph in the naked mole-rat. Nature. 1996, 380: 619-621. 10.1038/380619a0.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. O'Riain MJ, Jarvis JUM, Alexander R, Buffenstein R, Peeters C: Morphological castes in a vertebrate. PNAS. 2000, 97: 13194-13197. 10.1073/pnas.97.24.13194.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Scantlebury M, Speakman JR, Oosthuizen MK, Roper TJ, Bennett NC: Energetics reveals physiologically distinct castes in a eusocial mammal. Nature. 2006, 440: 795-797. 10.1038/nature04578.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Bennett NC, Faulkes CG, Molteno AJ: Reproductive suppression in subordinate, non-breeding female Damaraland mole-rats: Two components to a lifetime of socially induced infertility. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1996, 263: 1599-1603. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0234.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Clarke FM, Faulkes CC: Intracolony aggression in the eusocial naked mole-rat, Heterocephalus glaber. Animal Behaviour. 2001, 61: 311-324. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1573.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Velicer GJ, Yu YTN: Evolution of novel cooperative swarming in the bacterium Myxococcus xanthus. Nature. 2003, 425: 75-78. 10.1038/nature01908.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hardin G: Tragedy of commons. Science. 1968, 162: 1243-1248. 10.1126/science.162.3859.1243.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rankin DJ, Bargum K, Kokko H: The tragedy of the commons in evolutionary biology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2007, 22: 643-651.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Hamilton WD: Kinship, recognition, disease, and intelligence: constraints of social evolution. Animal Societies: Theories and Facts. Edited by: Ito J, Brown JL, Kikkawa K. 1987, Tokyo: Jpn Sci Soc, 81-102.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Queller DC: A general model for kin selection. Evolution. 1992, 46: 376-80. 10.2307/2409858.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Frank SA: Foundations of social evolution. 1998, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  43. Trivers RL: The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971, 46: 35-57. 10.1086/406755.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Frank SA: Repression of competition and the evolution of cooperation. Evolution. 2003, 57: 693-705.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. van Baalen M, Rand DA: The unit of selection in viscous populations and the evolution of altruism. J theor Biol. 1998, 143: 631-648. 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0730.

    Google Scholar 

  46. West SA, Pen I, Griffin AS: Cooperation and competition between relatives. Science. 2002, 296: 72-75. 10.1126/science.1065507.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Santos FC, Rodrigues JF, Pacheco JM: Graph topology plays a determinant role in the evolution of cooperation. Proc Royal Soc London B. 2006, 273: 51-55. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3272.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Grafen A: Do animals really recognize kin?. Anim Behav. 1990, 39: 42-54. 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80724-9.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Hochberg ME, Sinervo B, Brown SP: Socially mediation speciation. Evolution. 2003, 57: 154-158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Axelrod R, Hammond RA, Grafen A: Altruism via kin-selection strategies that rely on arbitrary tags with which they co-evolve. Evolution. 2004, 58: 1833-48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Crespi BJ: The evolution of social behavior in microorganism. Trends Ecol Evol. 2001, 16: 178-83. 10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02115-2.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Sachs J, Mueller IG, Wilcox TP, Bull JJ: The evolution of cooperation. Q Rev Biol. 2004, 79: 135-60. 10.1086/383541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Lehmann L, Keller L: The evolution of cooperation and altruism – a general framework and a classification of models. J Evol Biol. 2006, 19: 1365-1376. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01119.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. West SA, Griffin AS, Gardner A: Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology. 2007, 17: R661-R672. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Santelices B: How many kinds of individuals are there?. Trends Ecol Evol. 1999, 14: 152-155. 10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01519-5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Müller WEG: The origin of metazoan complexity: Porifera as integrated animals. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2003, 43: 3-10. 10.1093/icb/43.1.3.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Foster KR, Fortunato A, Strassmann JE, Queller DC: The costs and benefits of being a chimera. Proc R Soc London Ser B. 2002, 269: 2357-62. 10.1098/rspb.2002.2163.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Wilson DS, Sober E: Reviving the superorganism. J theor Biol. 1989, 136: 337-356. 10.1016/S0022-5193(89)80169-9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Gardner A, West SA, Barton NH: The relation between multilocus population genetics and social evolution theory. Am Nat. 2007, 169: 207-226. 10.1086/510602.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Keller L, Ed: Levels of Selection in Evolution. 1999, Princeton; Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  61. Helms Cahan S: Cooperation and conflict in social groups: insights from geographic variation. Anim Behav. 2001, 61: 819-825. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1671.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Taylor PD, Frank SA: How to make a kin selection model. J theor Biol. 1996, 180: 27-37. 10.1006/jtbi.1996.0075.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Helms Cahan S, Blumstein DT, Sundstrom L, Liebig J, Griffin A: Social trajectories and the evolution of social behavior. Oikos. 2002, 96: 206-216. 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2002.960202.x.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Bonner JT: Evolutionary strategies and developmental constraints in the cellular slime molds. American Naturalist. 1982, 119: 530-552. 10.1086/283930.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Nonacs P: A life-history approach to group living and social contracts between individuals. Ann Zool Fenn. 2001, 38: 239-254.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Taborsky M: The evolution of bourgeois, parasitic, and cooperative reproductive behaviors in fishes. J Heredity. 2001, 92: 100-110. 10.1093/jhered/92.2.100.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Roff DA: The Evolution of Life Histories. 1992, New York: Chapman & Hall

    Google Scholar 

  68. Zera AJ, Denno RF: Physiology and ecology of dispersal polymorphism in insects. Ann Rev Entom. 1997, 42: 207-230. 10.1146/annurev.ento.42.1.207.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Zera AJ, Harshman LG: The physiology of life history trade-offs in animals. Ann Rev Ecol Syst. 2001, 32: 95-126. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.32.081501.114006.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Zera AJ, Zhao ZW: Intermediary metabolism and life-history trade-offs: Differential metabolism of amino acids underlies the dispersal-reproduction trade-off in a wing-polymorphic cricket. Am Nat. 2006, 167: 889-900. 10.1086/503578.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Langellotto GA, Denno RF, Ott JR: A trade-off between flight capability and reproduction in males of a wing-dimorphic insect. Ecology. 2000, 81: 865-875.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Hughes CL, Hill JK, Dytham C: Evolutionary trade-offs between reproduction and dispersal in populations at expanding range boundaries. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2003, 270: S147-S150. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0049.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Frank SA: Kin selection and virulence in the evolution of protocells and parasites. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1994, 258: 153-161. 10.1098/rspb.1994.0156.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Avilés L, Fletcher JA, Cutter AD: The kin composition of social groups: Trading group size for degree of altruism. Am Nat. 2004, 164: 132-144. 10.1086/422263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Clutton-Brock TH: Behavioral ecology – Breeding together: Kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science. 2002, 296: 69-72. 10.1126/science.296.5565.69.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Gardner A, West SA: Demography, altruism, and the benefits of budding. J Evol Biol. 2006, 19: 1707-1716. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01104.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Maynard Smith J: Evolutionary progress and the levels of selection. Evolutionary Progress. Edited by: Nitecki MH. 1989, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 219-230.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Dugatkin LA: Cooperation Among Animals an Evolutionary Perspective. 1997, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  79. Rainey PB, Rainey K: Evolution of cooperation and conflict in experimental bacterial populations. Nature. 2003, 425: 72-74. 10.1038/nature01906.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Brockhurst MA, Hochberg ME, Bell T, Buckling A: Character displacement promotes cooperation in bacterial biofilms. Current Biology. 2006, 16: 2030-2034. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.068.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Hamilton WD, May RM: Dispersal in stable habitats. Nature. 1977, 269: 578-581. 10.1038/269578a0.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Frank SA: Dispersal polymorphisms in subdivided populations. J theor Biol. 1986, 122: 303-309. 10.1016/S0022-5193(86)80122-9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Hamilton WD: The genetical evolution of social behavior, I & II. J theor Biol. 1964, 1: 1-52. 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Lehmann L, Keller L, West S, Roze D: Group selection and kin selection: Two concepts but one process. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007, 104: 6736-6739. 10.1073/pnas.0700662104.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Taylor PD: Altruism in viscous populations – an inclusive fitness model. Evol Ecol. 1992, 6: 352-356. 10.1007/BF02270971.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Frank SA: Host-symbiont conflict over the mixing of symbiotic lineages. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1996, 263: 339-344. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0052.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  87. Dierkes P, Taborsky M, Kohler U: Reproductive parasitism of broodcare helpers in a cooperatively breeding fish. Behav Ecol. 1999, 10: 510-515. 10.1093/beheco/10.5.510.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Boyd R, Richerson PJ: Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethol Sociobiol. 1992, 13: 171-195. 10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-Y.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Hall-Stoodley L, Costerton JW, Stoodley P: Bacterial biofilms: From the natural environment to infectious diseases. Nature Reviews Microbiology. 2004, 2: 95-108. 10.1038/nrmicro821.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Xavier JB, Foster KR: Cooperation and conflict in microbial biofilms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007, 104: 876-881. 10.1073/pnas.0607651104.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Komdeur J: Variation in individual investment strategies among social animals. Ethology. 2006, 112: 729-747. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01243.x.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Hamilton WD: Altruism and related phenomena mainly in the social insects. Ann Rev Ecol Syst. 1972, 3: 193-232. 10.1146/annurev.es.03.110172.001205.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Fjerdingstad EJ, Schtickzelle N, Manhes P, Gutierrez A, Clobert J: Evolution of dispersal and life history strategies – Tetrahymena ciliates. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2007, 7: 133-10.1186/1471-2148-7-133.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Hamilton IM, Taborsky M: Unrelated helpers will not fully compensate for costs imposed on breeders when they pay to stay. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2005, 272: 445-454. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2961.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Hamilton IM, Taborsky M: Contingent movement and cooperation evolve under generalized reciprocity. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2005, 272: 2259-2267. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3248.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Velicer GJ, Kroos L, Lenski RE: Loss of social behaviors by Myxococcus xanthus during evolution in an unstructured habitat. PNAS. 1998, 95: 12376-12380. 10.1073/pnas.95.21.12376.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Strassmann JE, Zhu Y, Queller DC: Altruism and social cheating in the social amoeba Dictyostelium discoideum. Nature. 2000, 408: 965-967. 10.1038/35050087.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Gilbert O, Foster KR, Mehdiabadi NJ, Strassmann JE, Queller DC: High relatedness maintains multicellular cooperation in a social amoeba by controlling cheater mutants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2007, 104: 8913-8917. 10.1073/pnas.0702723104.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  99. Pineda-Krch M, Lehtilä K: Costs and benefits of genetic heterogeneity within organisms. J Evol Biol. 2004, 17: 1167-77. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00808.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Roze D, Rousset F: The robustness of Hamilton's rule with inbreeding and dominance: kin selection and fixation probabilities under partial sibmating. The American Naturalist. 2004, 164: 214-231. 10.1086/422202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Kamo M, Boots M: The evolution of parasite dispersal, transmission, and virulence in spatial host populations. Evol Ecol Res. 2006, 8: 1333-1347.

    Google Scholar 

  102. Ewald PW: Evolution of Infectious Disease. 1994, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  103. Frank SA: Models of parasite virulence. Q Rev Biol. 1996, 71: 37-78. 10.1086/419267.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Koella JC: The spatial spread of altruism versus the evolutionary response of egoists. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2000, 267: 1979-1985. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1239.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  105. Pepper JW, Smuts BB: A mechanism for the evolution of altruism among nonkin: Positive assortment through environmental feedback. Am Nat. 2002, 160: 205-213. 10.1086/341018.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Wilson DS, Dugatkin LA: Group selection and assortative interactions. Am Nat. 1997, 149: 336-351. 10.1086/285993.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Aktipis CA: Know when to walk away: contingent movement and the evolution of cooperation. J theor Biol. 2004, 231: 249-260. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.06.020.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Le Galliard J-F, Ferrière R, Dieckmann U: Adaptive evolution of social traits: Origin, trajectories, and correlations of altruism and mobility. Am Nat. 2005, 165: 206-224. 10.1086/427090.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Kirk DL: Seeking the ultimate and proximate causes of Volvox multicellularity and cellular differentiation. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2003, 43: 247-253. 10.1093/icb/43.2.247.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Rainey PB: Unity from conflict. Nature. 2007, 446: 616-10.1038/446616a.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Turner PE, Chao L: Sex and the evolution of intrahost competition in RNA virus Φ6. Genetics. 1998, 150: 523-532.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Turner PE, Chao L: Escape from prisoner's dilemma in RNA phage Φ6. American Naturalist. 2003, 161: 497-505. 10.1086/367880.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Chao L, Levin BR: Structured habitats and the evolution of anticompetitor toxins in bacteria. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1981, 78: 6324-6328. 10.1073/pnas.78.10.6324.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Riley MA, Wertz JE: Bacteriocins: evolution, ecology and application. Annual Review of Microbiology. 2002, 56: 117-137. 10.1146/annurev.micro.56.012302.161024.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Feldgarden M, Riley MA: The phenotypic and fitness effects of colicin resistance in Escherichia coli K-12. Evolution. 1999, 53: 1019-1027. 10.2307/2640807.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Klausen M, Gjermansen M, Kreft JU, Tolker-Nielsen T: Dynamics of development and dispersal in sessile microbial communities: examples from Pseudomonas aeruginosa and Pseudomonas putida model biofilms. Fems Microbiology Letters. 2006, 261: 1-11. 10.1111/j.1574-6968.2006.00280.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Bantinaki E, Kassen R, Knight CG, Robinson Z, Spiers AJ, Rainey PB: Adaptive divergence in experimental populations of Pseudomonas fluorescens. III. mutational origins of wrinkly spreader diversity. Genetics. 2007, 176: 441-453. 10.1534/genetics.106.069906.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Spormann AM: Gliding motility in bacteria: insights from studies of Myxococcus xanthus. Microbiol Mol Biol Rev. 1999, 63: 621-641.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Fiegna F, Velicer GJ: Exploitative and hierarchical antagonism in a cooperative bacterium. Plos Biology. 2005, 3: 1980-1987. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030370.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  120. Fiegna F, Yu YTN, Kadam SV, Velicer GJ: Evolution of an obligate social cheater to a superior cooperator. Nature. 2006, 441: 310-314. 10.1038/nature04677.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Vos M, Velicer GJ: Genetic population structure of the soil bacterium Myxococcus xanthus at the centimeter scale. Applied and Environmental Microbiology. 2006, 72: 3615-3625. 10.1128/AEM.72.5.3615-3625.2006.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Sesti F, Shih TM, Nikolaeva N, Goldstein SA: Immunity to K1 killer toxin: internal TOK1 blockade. Cell. 2001, 105: 637-644. 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00376-2.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Greig D, Travisano M: The Prisoner's Dilemma and polymorphism in yeast SUC genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 2004, 271: S25-S26. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0083.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  124. Greig D, Travisano M: The prisoner's dilemma and polymorphism in yeast SUC genes. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2004, 271: S25-S26. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0083.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  125. MacLean RC, Gudelj I: Resource competition and social conflict in experimental populations of yeast. Nature. 2006, 441: 498-501. 10.1038/nature04624.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Filosa MF: Heterocytosis in cellular slime molds. American Naturalist. 1962, 96: 73-91.

    Google Scholar 

  127. Buss LW: Somatic cell parasitism and the evolution of somatic tissue compatibility. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America-Biological Sciences. 1982, 79: 5337-5341. 10.1073/pnas.79.17.5337.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  128. Dao DN, Kessin RH, Ennis HL: Developmental cheating and the evolutionary biology of Dictyostelium and Myxococcus. Microbiology-Uk. 2000, 146: 1505-1512.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  129. Ennis HL, Dao DN, Pukatzki SU, Kessin RH: Dictyostelium amoebae lacking an F-box protein form spores rather than stalk in chimeras with wild type. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000, 97: 3292-3297. 10.1073/pnas.050005097.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  130. Hilson JA, Kolmes SA, Nellis LF: Fruiting body architecture, spore capsule contents, selfishness, and heterocytosis in the cellular slime mold Dictyostelium discoideum. EtholEcolEvol. 1994, 6: 529-535.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Fortunato A, Strassmann JE, Santorelli L, Queller DC: Co-occurrence in nature of different clones of the social amoeba, Dictyostelium discoideum. Molecular Ecology. 2003, 12: 1031-1038. 10.1046/j.1365-294X.2003.01792.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Queller DC, Foster KR, Fortunato A, Strassmann JE, Kikuchi T, Kubo T, Higashi S: Cooperation and conflict in the social amoeba, Dictyostelium discoideum. Social insects and sociogenetics. 2003, Sapporo: Hokkaido University Press, 173-200.

    Google Scholar 

  133. Castillo DI, Switz GT, Foster KR, Queller DC, Strassmann JE: A cost to chimerism in Dictyostelium discoideum on natural substrates. Evolutionary Ecology Research. 2005, 7: 263-271.

    Google Scholar 

  134. Kuzdzal-Fick JJ, Foster KR, Queller DC, Strassmann JE: Exploiting new terrain: an advantage to sociality in the slime mold Dictyostelium discoideum. Behavioral Ecology. 2007, 18: 433-437. 10.1093/beheco/arl102.

    Google Scholar 

  135. Santorelli LA, Thompson CRL, Villegas e, Svetz J, Dinh C, Parikh A, Sucgang R, Kuspal A, Strassmann JE, Queller DC, Shaulsky G: Facultative cheater mutants reveal the genetic complexity of cooperation in social amoebae. Nature. 2008, doi:10.1038/nature06558

    Google Scholar 

  136. Darden WH: Sexual differentiation in Volvox aureus. Journal of Protozoology. 1966, 13: 239-255.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  137. Kirk DL: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cellular differentiation in Volvox. Trends in Genetics. 1988, 4: 32-36. 10.1016/0168-9525(88)90063-7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Kirk DL: The genetic program for germ-soma differentiation in Volvox. Annual Review of Genetics. 1997, 31: 359-380. 10.1146/annurev.genet.31.1.359.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. Kirk DL: Seeking the ultimate and proximate causes of Volvox multicellularity and cellular differentiation. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2003, 43: 247-253. 10.1093/icb/43.2.247.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  140. Koufopanou V: The evolution of soma in the Volvocales. American Naturalist. 1994, 143: 907-931. 10.1086/285639.

    Google Scholar 

  141. Desnitski AG: A review on the evolution of development in Volvox: morphological and physiological aspects. European Journal of Protistology. 1995, 31: 241-247.

    Google Scholar 

  142. Nedelcu AM, Michod RE: The evolutionary origin of an altruistic gene. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 2006, 23: 1460-1464. 10.1093/molbev/msl016.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  143. Solari CA, Kessler JO, Michod RE: A hydrodynamics approach to the evolution of multicellularity: Flagellar motility and germ-soma differentiation in volvocalean green algae. American Naturalist. 2006, 167: 537-554. 10.1086/501031.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Michod RE: Evolution of individuality during the transition from unicellular to multicellular life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007, 104: 8613-8618. 10.1073/pnas.0701489104.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  145. Leadbeater BSC: Life-history and ultrastructure of a new marine species of Proterospongia (Choanoflagellida). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom. 1983, 63: 135-160.

    Google Scholar 

  146. Blackstone NW: Redox control and the evolution of multicellularity. Bioessays. 2000, 22: 947-953. 10.1002/1521-1878(200010)22:10<947::AID-BIES10>3.0.CO;2-W.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  147. Maldonado M: Choanoflagellates, choanocytes, and animal multicellularity. Invertebrate Biology. 2004, 123: 1-22.

    Google Scholar 

  148. Kaestner A: Lehrbuch der speziellen Zoologie; Wirbellose. 1969, Stuttgart: Fischer Verlag, 1:

    Google Scholar 

  149. Galtsoff PS: The amoeboid movement of dissociated sponge cells. Biological Bulletin. 1923, 45: 153-161. 10.2307/1536625.

    Google Scholar 

  150. Kuhns WJ, Weinbaum G, Turner R, Burger MM: Sponge aggregation: model for studies on cell-cell interactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1974, 234: 58-74. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1974.tb53021.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  151. Simpson TL: The cell biology of sponges. 1984, New York: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  152. Usher KM, Sutton DC, Toze S, Kuo J, Fromont J: Sexual reproduction in Chondrilla australiensis (Porifera: demospongiae). Marine and Freshwater Research. 2004, 55: 123-134. 10.1071/MF03058.

    Google Scholar 

  153. Degnan BM, Leys SP, Larroux C: Sponge development and antiquity of animal pattern formation. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2005, 45: 335-341. 10.1093/icb/45.2.335.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Whalan S, Johnson MS, Harvey E, Battershill C: Mode of reproduction, recruitment, and genetic subdivision in the brooding sponge Haliclona sp. Marine Biology. 2005, 146: 425-433. 10.1007/s00227-004-1466-8.

    Google Scholar 

  155. Agata K, Nakajima E, Funayama N, Shibata N, Saito YUY: Two different evolutionary origins of stem cell systems and their molecular basis. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2006, 17: 503-509. 10.1016/j.semcdb.2006.05.004.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  156. Müller W: Experimentelle untersuchungen uber stockentwicklung, polypendifferenzierung und sexualchimaren bei Hydractinia echinata. Roux' Archiv fur Ent. 1964, 155: 181-268. 10.1007/BF00573905.

    Google Scholar 

  157. Buss LW, Shenk MA, Marchalonis JJ, Reinish C: Hydroid allorecognition regulates competition at both the level of the colony and the level of the cell lineage. Defense molecules. 1990, New York: Liss, 85-105.

    Google Scholar 

  158. Shenk MA, Buss LW: Ontogenetic changes in fusibility in the colonial hydroid Hydractinia symbiolongicarpus. J Exp Zool. 1991, 257: 80-86. 10.1002/jez.1402570111.

    Google Scholar 

  159. Grosberg RK, Levitan DR, Cameron BB: Evolutionary genetics of allorecognition in the colonial marine hydroid Hydractinia symbiolongicarpus. Evolution. 1996, 50: 2221-2240. 10.2307/2410693.

    Google Scholar 

  160. Cartwright P: Developmental insights into the origin of complex colonial Hydrozoans. Integr Comp Biol. 2003, 43: 82-86. 10.1093/icb/43.1.82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Francis L: Social organization within clones of sea-anemone Anthopleura elegantissima. Biological Bulletin. 1976, 150: 361-376. 10.2307/1540678.

    Google Scholar 

  162. Ayre DJ, Grosberg RK: Behind anemone lines: factors affecting division of labour in the social cnidarian Anthopleura elegantissima. Animal Behaviour. 2005, 70: 97-110. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.022.

    Google Scholar 

  163. Silen L: Zur kenntnis des Polymorphismus der Bryozoen. Die avicularien der Cheilostomata Anasca. Zool Bidr Upps. 1938, 17: 149-366.

    Google Scholar 

  164. Silen L, Woollacott RM, Zimmer RL: Polymorphism. Biology of Bryozoans. 1977, New York: Academic Press, 183-231.

    Google Scholar 

  165. Ryland JS, Hayward PJ: British Anascan Bryozoans. 1977, London: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  166. Harvell CD: The evolution of polymorphism in colonial invertebrates and social insects. Quarterly Review of Biology. 1994, 69: 155-185. 10.1086/418538.

    Google Scholar 

  167. Sabbadin A, Zaniolo G: Sexual differentiation and germ cell transfer in the colonial ascidian Botryllus schlosseri. J Exp Zool. 1979, 207: 289-304. 10.1002/jez.1402070212.

    Google Scholar 

  168. Pancer Z, Gershon H, Rinkevich B: Coexistence and possible parasitism of somatic and germ-cell lines in chimeras of the colonial urochordate Botryllus schlosseri. Biological Bulletin. 1995, 189: 106-112. 10.2307/1542460.

    Google Scholar 

  169. Stoner DS, Rinkevich B, Weissman IL: Heritable germ and somatic cell lineage competitions in chimeric colonial protochordates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1999, 96: 9148-9153. 10.1073/pnas.96.16.9148.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  170. Rinkevich B, Yankelevich I: Environmental split between germ cell parasitism and somatic cell synergism in chimeras of a colonial urochordate. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2004, 207: 3531-3536. 10.1242/jeb.01184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  171. Hartl DL, Hiraizumi Y, Crow JF: Evidence for sperm dysfunction as mechanism of segretation distortion in Drosophila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1967, 58: 2240-2245. 10.1073/pnas.58.6.2240.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  172. Hiraizumi Y, Thomas AM: Suppressor systems of segregation distorter (SD) chromosomes in natural populations of Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 1984, 106: 279-292.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  173. Merrill C, Bayraktaroglu L, Kusano A, Ganetzky B: Truncated RanGAP encoded by the segregation distorter locus of Drosophila. Science. 1999, 283: 1742-1745. 10.1126/science.283.5408.1742.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  174. Atlan A, Mercot H, Landre C, Montchamp-Moreau C: The sex-ratio trait in Drosophila simulans: Geographical distribution of distortion and resistance. Evolution. 1997, 51: 1886-1895. 10.2307/2411010.

    Google Scholar 

  175. Silver LM: Mouse T-haplotypes. Annual Review of Genetics. 1985, 19: 179-208.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  176. Gummere GR, McCormick PJ, Bennett D: The influence of genetic background and the homologous chromosome-17 on T-haplotype transmission ratio distortion in mice. Genetics. 1986, 114: 235-245.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  177. Ardlie KG: Putting the brake on drive: meiotic drive of t haplotypes in natural populations of mice. Trends in Genetics. 1998, 14: 189-193. 10.1016/S0168-9525(98)01455-3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  178. Korb J, Schmidinger S: Help or disperse? Cooperation in termites influenced by food conditions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004, 56: 89-95. 10.1007/s00265-004-0757-x.

    Google Scholar 

  179. Roux EA, Korb J: Evolution of eusociality and the soldier caste in termites: a validation of the intrinsic benefit hypothesis. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2004, 17: 869-875. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00727.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  180. Crespi BJ: Eusociality in Australian gall thrips. Nature. 1992, 359: 724-726. 10.1038/359724a0.

    Google Scholar 

  181. Chapman TW, Crespi B: High relatedness and inbreeding in two species of haplodiploid eusocial thrips (Insecta: Thysanoptera) revealed by microsatellite analysis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1998, 43: 301-306. 10.1007/s002650050495.

    Google Scholar 

  182. Chapman TW, Crespi BJ, Kranz BD, Schwarz MP: High relatedness and inbreeding at the origin of eusociality in gall-inducing thrips. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000, 97: 1648-1650. 10.1073/pnas.020510097.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  183. Chapman TW, Kranz BD, Bejah KL, Morris DC, Schwarz MP, Crespi BJ: The evolution of soldier reproduction in social thrips. Behavioral Ecology. 2002, 13: 519-525. 10.1093/beheco/13.4.519.

    Google Scholar 

  184. Wills TE, Chapman TW, Kranz BD, Schwarz MP: Reproductive division of labour coevolves with gall size in Australian thrips with soldiers. Naturwissenschaften. 2001, 88: 526-529. 10.1007/s00114-001-0273-z.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  185. Foster WA: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1990, 27: 421-430. 10.1007/BF00164069.

    Google Scholar 

  186. Stern DL, Foster WA: The evolution of soldiers in aphids. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 1996, 71: 27-79. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1996.tb00741.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  187. Abbot P, Withgott JH, Moran NA: Genetic conflict and conditional altruism in social aphid colonies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001, 98: 12068-12071. 10.1073/pnas.201212698.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  188. Johnson PCD, Whitfield JA, Foster WA, Amos W: Clonal mixing in the soldier-producing aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae (Hemiptera: Aphididae). Molecular Ecology. 2002, 11: 1525-1531. 10.1046/j.1365-294X.2002.01530.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  189. Ratnieks FLW, Visscher PK: Worker policing in the honeybee. Nature. 1989, 342: 796-797. 10.1038/342796a0.

    Google Scholar 

  190. Visscher PK: A quantitative study of worker reproduction in honeybee colonies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1989, 25: 247-254. 10.1007/BF00300050.

    Google Scholar 

  191. Visscher PK: Reproductive conflict in honey bees: A stalemate of worker egg-laying and policing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1996, 39: 237-244. 10.1007/s002650050286.

    Google Scholar 

  192. Barron AB, Oldroyd BP, Ratnieks FLW: Worker reproduction in honey-bees (Apis) and the anarchic syndrome: a review. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2001, 50: 199-208. 10.1007/s002650100362.

    Google Scholar 

  193. Ratnieks FLW, Foster KR, Wenseleers T: Conflict resolution in insect societies. Annual Review of Entomology. 2006, 51: 581-608. 10.1146/annurev.ento.51.110104.151003.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  194. Imperatriz-Fonseca VL, Zucchi R: Virgin queens in stingless bee (Apidae, Meliponinae) colonies – a review. Apidologie. 1995, 26: 231-244. 10.1051/apido:19950305.

    Google Scholar 

  195. Peters JM, Queller DC, Imperatriz-Fonseca VL, Roubik DW, Strassmann JE: Mate number, kin selection and social conflicts in stingless bees and honeybees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1999, 266: 379-384. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0648.

    Google Scholar 

  196. Taborsky M: Broodcare helpers in the cichlid fish Lamprologus-brichardi – their costs and benefits. Anim Behav. 1984, 32: 1236-1252. 10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80241-9.

    Google Scholar 

  197. Dierkes P, Heg D, Taborsky M, Skubic E, Achmann R: Genetic relatedness in groups is sex-specific and declines with age of helpers in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Ecology Letters. 2005, 8: 968-975. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00801.x.

    Google Scholar 

  198. Heg D, Bachar Z: Cooperative breeding in the Lake Tanganyika cichlid Julidochromis ornatus. Environmental Biology of Fishes. 2006, 76: 265-281. 10.1007/s10641-006-9032-5.

    Google Scholar 

  199. Heg D, Bachar Z, Brouwer L, Taborsky M: Predation risk is an ecological constraint for helper dispersal in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2004, 271: 2367-2374. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2855.

    Google Scholar 

  200. Rowley I: Communal activities among white-winged choughs Corcorax melanorhamphos. Ibis. 1978, 120: 178-197. 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1978.tb06774.x.

    Google Scholar 

  201. Heinsohn R, Cockburn A: Helping is costly to young birds in cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1994, 256: 293-298. 10.1098/rspb.1994.0083.

    Google Scholar 

  202. Heinsohn R, Dunn P, Legge S, Double M: Coalitions of relatives and reproductive skew in cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2000, 267: 243-249. 10.1098/rspb.2000.0993.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  203. Heinsohn RG: Cooperative enhancement of reproductive success in white-winged choughs. Evolutionary Ecology. 1992, 6: 97-114. 10.1007/BF02270705.

    Google Scholar 

  204. Boland CRJ, Heinsohn R, Cockburn A: Deception by helpers in cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs and its experimental manipulation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1997, 41: 251-256. 10.1007/s002650050386.

    Google Scholar 

  205. Jarvis JUM: Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 1981, 212: 571-573. 10.1126/science.7209555.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  206. Reeve HK: Queen activation of lazy workers in colonies of the eusocial naked mole-rat. Nature. 1992, 358: 147-149. 10.1038/358147a0.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  207. Jarvis JUM, Bennett NC: Eusociality has evolved independently in 2 genera of bathyergid mole-rats, but occurs in no other subterranean mammal. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1993, 33: 253-260. 10.1007/BF02027122.

    Google Scholar 

  208. Jarvis JUM, O'Riain MJ, Bennett NC, Sherman PW: Mammalian eusociality: a family affair. Trends EcolEvol. 1994, 9: 46-51.

    Google Scholar 

  209. Bennett NC, Faulkes CG: African Mole-rats Ecology and Eusociality. 2000, Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press

    Google Scholar 

  210. Burland TM, Bennett NC, Jarvis JUM, Faulkes CG: Eusociality in African mole-rats: new insights from patterns of genetic relatedness in the Damaraland mole-rat (Cryptomys damarensis). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2002, 269: 1025-1030. 10.1098/rspb.2002.1978.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Daniel Blumstein, Ross Crozier, Steve Frank, Toby Kiers, Barbara Taborsky, Peter Taylor and Don Waller for helpful discussions, and Steve Frank, Andy Gardner, Paul Rainey, Stuart West and two anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier drafts. MEH acknowledges the Santa Fe Institute (2005), the National Center of Ecological Analysis and Research (2006–2007), and the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique for financial support. MT and DJR acknowledge the Swiss National Science Foundation for support (SNF-grant 3100A0-105626).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael E Hochberg.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

MEH conceived the study, developed and analyzed the model and wrote the manuscript. DJR participated in the design of the study, developed the model and participated in writing the manuscript. MT participated in the design of the study, constructed Table S1, and participated in writing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12862_2008_807_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: (Table S1). Examples of group formation for which there is some information on dispersal, relatedness and punishment/policing. (DOC 95 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hochberg, M.E., Rankin, D.J. & Taborsky, M. The coevolution of cooperation and dispersal in social groups and its implications for the emergence of multicellularity. BMC Evol Biol 8, 238 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-8-238

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-8-238

Keywords