Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Context-specific close-range “hoo” calls in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar)

Abstract

Background

Close range calls are produced by many animals during intra-specific interactions, such as during home range defence, playing, begging for food, and directing others. In this study, we investigated the most common close range vocalisation of lar gibbons (Hylobates lar), the ‘hoo’ call. Gibbons and siamangs (family Hylobatidae) are known for their conspicuous and elaborate songs, while quieter, close range vocalisations have received almost no empirical attention, perhaps due to the difficult observation conditions in their natural forest habitats.

Results

We found that ‘hoo’ calls were emitted by both sexes in a variety of contexts, including feeding, separation from group members, encountering predators, interacting with neighbours, or as part of duet songs by the mated pair. Acoustic analyses revealed that ‘hoo’ calls varied in a number of spectral parameters as a function of the different contexts. Males’ and females’ ‘hoo’ calls showed similar variation in these context-specific parameter differences, although there were also consistent sex differences in frequency across contexts.

Conclusions

Our study provides evidence that lar gibbons are able to generate significant, context-dependent acoustic variation within their main social call, which potentially allows recipients to make inferences about the external events experienced by the caller. Communicating about different events by producing subtle acoustic variation within some call types appears to be a general feature of primate communication, which can increase the expressive power of vocal signals within the constraints of limited vocal tract flexibility that is typical for all non-human primates. In this sense, this study is of direct relevance for the on-going debate about the nature and origins of vocally-based referential communication and the evolution of human speech.

Background

Primate vocal behaviour has been extensively studied, in recent years often with the aim of examining the cognitive underpinnings and evolutionary relationship to human language. Although language is a uniquely human behaviour, it is likely to have evolved from precursors in the primate lineage, some of which may still be detectable in the vocal behaviour of extant primates [1]. One important candidate for such a precursor is the ability to produce context-specific calls, a prerequisite to referential communication during which an actor refers a recipient’s attention to an external event. In animal communication, this is sometimes known as ‘functionally referential’ communication because it is usually not known whether or not such communication is intentional [2]. The classic example is the predator alarm call system of vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops) that produce acoustically distinct calls for different classes of predators, such as eagles, snakes and leopards [3,4]. Because vervet alarm calls can elicit distinct and adaptive anti-predator behaviours in listeners, even in the absence of a predator, it has been argued that the calls are meaningful to other monkeys, with some resemblance to human words or phrases [5]. More recently, functionally referential calling behaviour also has been described for other species of monkeys [6-9], apes [10-14], dogs [15], dolphins [16], and birds such as fowl [17], jays [18] and chickadees [19].

Overall, context-specific calling behaviour appears to be widespread in animal communication, presumably because the selection pressure to attend to and understand context-specific calls is very strong, especially in evolutionarily urgent situations. In addition, there is good evidence for call comprehension between different species of primates [20,21], between primates and birds [22] and between primates and other mammals [23], suggesting that such phenomena are driven by a generalised cognitive mechanism that is widely available to animals. Whether or not such abilities are relevant for understanding language evolution has triggered much debate with no real consensus [24,25]. Nevertheless, the comparative study of animal communication, especially across non-human primates, is one of the most useful tools to make progress and address open questions about human language evolution [26].

From an evolutionary perspective, context-specific communication is puzzling, mainly because it is not immediately clear why callers should provide accurate information about external events that they have witnessed. This is especially the case if the signals are costly to the caller, for example by attracting the attention of a predator or competitors, as in the case of alarm and food calls. Kin selection offers a plausible explanation [27], but this is relevant only if the caller can directly or indirectly benefit genetic relatives and enhance their chances of survival and reproduction. In primates this is often the case as most species tend to live in individualised social groups with a well-defined membership, suggesting that individuals regularly interact with individuals that are closely related to them [28].

Following the evolutionary logic of kin selection of context-specific calls, it is therefore surprising that primate vocal repertoires are generally small, consisting of only a handful of acoustically distinct call types [29], which is also true for human’s closest living relatives, the chimpanzees [30]. On the other hand, vocal repertoire sizes are often underestimated because many species appear to be able to generate subtle variations within some call types and because of combinatorial phenomena [31]. Both acoustic variants and signal combinations effectively increase a species’ repertoire size and therefore its expressive power. Recent examples are Diana monkey contact calls, which despite overall uniformity vary subtly according to social context [32], and chimpanzees and bonobos food-associated calls, which vary with food quality [13,33,34]. Subtle acoustic changes in otherwise identical call types may therefore function to counter the physical constraints of the generally inflexible non-human primate vocal tract.

In this study, we are interested in the variation of close-range ‘hoo’ calls of another ape species, the lar gibbon (Hylobates lar). Gibbons are mainly known for their loud and conspicuous songs, audible over long distances, which allow callers to communicate beyond their immediate home ranges in dense forest habitat [35,36]. In earlier work, we have presented observational evidence for functionally referential communication in these songs [37] and recent studies have compared the convergent development of patterns for human speech and human singing with gibbon song [38,39].

Lar gibbons also produce a number of soft call types, which so far have not attracted much attention. In a pioneering field study CR Carpenter [40] already alluded to close range calls in lar gibbons, which were described as “…low volume sounds” impossible to record with the technology available at the time. He described the entire vocal repertoire as consisting of nine distinct call types with five of them confined to within-group communication. These within-group calls functioned in cohesion, defence, play, begging, and directing others. JO Ellefson [41] labelled one call the “hoo” and described it as a “…broad pitch range soft to medium loud, being emitted singly or in short bursts of two or three per second” (p. 128). According to Ellefson (1974), hoos were produced in a variety of contexts, such as short distance separation from group members, responding to a human observer, encountering preferred food (“glug-hoo”), and during encounters with neighbouring groups (“conflict-hoo”). Although Ellefson speculated that hoos might show systematic acoustic variation across contexts, to our knowledge this hypothesis has never been tested empirically. In addition, JJ Raemaekers, PM Raemaekers and EH Haimoff [42] noted that hoos could also be part of H. lar songs, described as having a “…short, uninflected and steeply rising” shape, with a lower pitch and narrower frequency range compared to closely related “wa” calls. In a previous study [37] we confirmed these preliminary observations and suggested that “hoos” should be classed as a distinct “note” produced as a prelude to, as well as a part of, more complex songs. Because hoos are used in so many contexts there are reasons to believe that the acoustic distinctions alluded to by Ellefson [41] are context-specific and potentially meaningful to conspecifics. This study is a first step in determining whether these calls are consistently distinct and could therefore be used in functionally referential communication.

In this article, we focussed on lar gibbon hoos given in several main contexts: feeding, within group separation, alert (usually after hearing an inter-specific alarm call or some other disturbance), predator encounters, inter-group encounters, and as a prelude to duet singing. Given the adaptive value of context-specific vocal signals and the well documented comprehension abilities in primates, we predicted that we would find context-specific acoustic variations especially between some of the evolutionarily important events such as predator and other group encounters.

Results

Sample size

EC spent 117 days following the gibbons amounting to approximately 600 hours of observation. From these we extracted a total of 462 hoos from 14 males and eleven females across nine different contexts and subjected them to acoustic analyses. Sample sizes for alert, separation and snake contexts were too small to be included in the main analyses (Table 1). For the females, the inter-group encounter context was omitted since females often remained passive and spatially removed/peripheral during intergroup encounters, while males engaged and interacted with neighbouring individuals. More importantly, females typically did not produce hoo vocalisations during encounter situations.

Table 1 Individual males and females in each context that contributed to the final analyses
Table 2 Statistical comparison of male and female hoo call contexts

We found that some acoustic parameters were easier to extract than others, which resulted in unequal sample sizes. For example, intensity was particularly difficult to measure, which yielded a smaller sample size than call duration, inter-call interval, and peak, low and delta frequency, which limited the number of possible comparisons.

Sex differences

Gibbons lack an obvious sexual dimorphism in body size, possibly due to their sometimes monogamous grouping structure [43]. While Khao Yai gibbons show stable polyandrous groupings as well as monogamous ones [44], we do not predict any differences in vocal behaviour. We found that the average peak frequency of hoos was 521.9Hz ± 3.79SE, (n = 462) with an average duration of 0.08 s ±0.002SE (n = 462). Surprisingly, however, we also found that male hoos had significantly higher peak frequencies (F0) and low frequencies than female hoos (Table 2, Figure 1). No sex differences were found in other acoustic parameters.

Table 3 Results of discriminant function analysis on hoo call contexts for males and females
Figure 1
figure 1

a-f. Box plots of the mean values for each measured acoustic parameter (not accounting for individual identity). Panel a) duration, b) intercall interval, c) peak frequency, d) low frequency, e) delta frequency, f) intensity. Error bars represent 95% CI. Different letters above box plots indicate significant differences between contexts. Where sex differences were found plots are split into two panels: F = female, M = male.

Context-specific call variants

For both males and females, Linear Mixed Models (LMMs) revealed there were significant differences in acoustic parameters across the seven different contexts (Table 3, Figure 1).

We then conducted a step-wise discriminant function analysis, entering the five most readily measurable acoustic parameters (peak frequency, low frequency, delta frequency, duration and inter-call interval) to determine whether the different contexts could be separated by the interactions between the measured variables. Since sex differences were frequency-related we conducted separate DFAs for males and females. Also, since hoos to leopard and tiger models did not differ from each other, these were pooled as “big cat” responses. Sample sizes for feeding hoos proved to be too small for the DFA when we split males and females. Similarly, intensity was excluded because sample sizes became too low (<20) when this variable was entered and sexes were split. Interestingly, intensity strongly predicted hoo context in initial analyses, making it particularly relevant for future study. The DFA results are summarised in Tables 3 and 4 and Figure 2.

Table 4 Discriminant function analysis classification table for males and females
Table 5 Behavioural contexts recorded during production of hoo calls
Figure 2
figure 2

Two scatter plots of functions 1 (intercall interval) and 2 (low frequency/delta frequency) as they explain the variation between the different hoo contexts in males (M) and females (F).  = big cat; □ = raptor;  = group encounter ∆ = duet; ■ = group centroid.

Discussion

We investigated the close-range hoo calls of free-ranging lar gibbons in a natural forest habitat of North-eastern Thailand to establish whether there were acoustic differences between the various contexts they were emitted in. Close-range calls of wild gibbons have not received much attention nor have they previously been analysed in detail, in contrast to countless studies on their singing behaviour [42,45-47]. Results revealed that both adult males and females produced context-specific hoo calls, according to the following patterns. First, both sexes were not distinguishable in the acoustic structure of their hoo calls given in specific contexts. For example, measures of peak and low frequency showed that the hoos given in response to raptors were significantly lower than the hoos given as part of a daily duet song, for both males and females. Furthermore, raptor hoos showed significant differences in a number of acoustic parameters compared to many of the other contexts: they were significantly shorter in duration than duet and leopard hoos for both males and females and raptor hoo intensity was significantly lower than the duet hoos, leopard hoos and tiger hoos. In addition, inter-call intervals were significantly longer for raptor hoos than hoos in other contexts, such as tiger, duet, leopard (males and females), and encounter hoos (males only). In fact, raptor hoos were acoustically distinct from all other context, including leopards and tigers, suggesting that they can be classified as a predator-specific call variant. This is in contrast to gibbon loud vocal responses to other predators [37,48]. Due to the gibbons’ relatively small body size raptors are a real threat to infants and juveniles, but probably not to mature gibbons, which may explain the different anti-predator responses by adult gibbons to raptors and cats [48]. Raptor hoos are occasionally also emitted by non-adults and it would therefore be interesting to acoustically compare non-adult and adult hoos in a future study. Raptor hoos were less intense, more widely spread out, and of shorter duration, lower frequency and smaller frequency span than the other hoos, making them the least audible of all hoo variants. This is consistent with the interpretation that raptor hoos are given in circumstances the gibbons may perceive less threatening or to prevent attracting the attention of the predator. For example some passerine bird raptor alarm “seet” calls are difficult to localise [49]. Raptors hear best in the range of 1-4 kHz [50,51], with sensitivity to sounds below and above this being much poorer than primates. Of note, gibbon hoos are consistently below the 1 kHz threshold, with raptor hoos being the lowest frequency of all. The gibbons’ relatively cryptic responses to different predators is consistent with what has been reported in other primates [52,53].

We also found further differences between other hoo contexts. For example, duet hoos tended to be higher in frequency than the other contexts, significantly so when compared with feeding, leopard, tiger and raptor hoos (males) and raptor and tiger hoos (females). The delta frequency of the tiger and leopard hoos tended to be greater than in feeding hoos (females). For encounters, we only obtained recordings from the males, since females did not usually engage in inter-group encounters, but here we also found significant differences in frequency measures if compared to the tiger, leopard and feeding context and significant differences in delta frequency if compared to tiger hoos. The only two contexts that did not differ significantly from one another were tiger and leopard hoos, suggesting that callers perceived these two predators as belonging to the same class.

Apart from contextual effects we also found some significant sex differences in the acoustic structure of hoo calls, despite the absence of sexual dimorphism in this species. Overall, female hoo calls had a significantly lower peak frequency than male hoo calls, in line with an earlier study [42]. Male hoo low frequencies were also significantly higher than female hoos, but we found no differences in delta frequency or intensity between the sexes.

Overall, the data presented here show that the differences between males and females are mainly in terms of frequency-related parameters, while the way that both sexes vocalise to contexts is identical. Among mammals in general, males tend to have lower frequency voices than females, possibly due to larger body size [54] so our findings are surprising. In gibbons, female voices are lower in frequency than males, despite the absence of obvious differences in body size across gibbon species [43]. One possibility is that subtle differences in H. lar body mass may be responsible: males are 0.56 kg (9%) heavier than females [55], though we would expect the opposite relationship with voice frequency if this slight difference in mass was important. Another possibility is that testosterone levels are partly responsible; the singing voices of adult male gibbons with higher testosterone have higher F0 [56], and since males typically have higher testosterone than females, this may better explain our findings. However, the ontogeny of gibbon voice qualities and their relationship with steroid hormones in both sexes require further research.

A DFA revealed intercall interval separated raptor hoos from big cat and duet hoos (females and males), and also encounter hoos (males). Low frequency separated big cat and raptor from duet hoos (females) and delta frequency separated duet, encounter and raptor from big cats (males). Overall, intercall intervals separated big cat and raptor hoos (males and females), while frequency-related measures separated duet from big cat hoos (males and females). For all contexts, there was also considerable overlap, suggesting that other acoustic variables may also contribute to distinguishing call context. For example, intensity appeared to be important but for reasons explained earlier, we omitted it from the final DFA. Also, without being able to fully control for the effects of distance from vocaliser to microphone and amount of vegetation, which could attenuate vocal signals, we remain cautious in interpreting the role of intensity in gibbon hoo communication. A larger sample size of excellent quality hoo recordings would be necessary to explore this preliminary finding.

In future work, it will be important to carry out systematic playback experiments of different hoo variants to determine whether this call type functions as a referential system in the way that their long-distance songs do [37].

The hoo calls described in this paper are virtually indistinguishable to human observers and call variants became only apparent in statistical analyses. Gibbon hoos should therefore be classed as an example of an acoustically graded call type, alongside other primate vocalisations, such as chimpanzee pant hoots [14] or chacma baboon grunts [57]. Social variables, such as an individual’s physical condition or social status, might additionally influence some of the acoustic parameters, which could explain some of the overlap seen in Figure 2. Since hoos are produced in multiple contexts, the motivational states of the callers will likely vary between them, which is particularly the case in predator hoos and inter-group encounter hoos. Some authors interpret such findings as evidence that animal signals do not carry any “meaning” [58,59], but that they serve to influence rather than inform listeners, which subsequently can become conventionalised using linguistic pragmatics [58]. However, while attention-getting vocalisations may not be designed to inform, it is difficult to argue that they do not do so [60], which in our opinion make such findings relevant to understand the origins of “meaningful” communication, including language [26].

Given the obvious selective advantage of ‘labelling’ contexts with distinct signals, why do gibbons and other non-human primates not produce acoustically more discrete signals for different contexts, instead of the subtle acoustic variations within just one call type? We can think of the following possibilities. Firstly, non-human primates may generally be prevented from the required vocal tract control to produce more discrete signals, due to anatomical and neurological constraints, the classic argument to explain limitations in vocal flexibility and control in non-human primates [61]. Gibbons are not alone in this, as there is increasing evidence that subtle changes within the main basic call types can generate meaningful information (see [62]). Secondly, the acoustic variation seen in gibbon hoos (and in other primate vocalisation types) may simply be the basis of acoustic flexibility, similar to human speech, in which subtle acoustic parameters, like pitch, can be important carriers of meaning (e.g. Chinese or Thai: [63]).

Context-specific calling behaviour appears to be widespread and therefore was likely present in the ancestor of modern primates and humans. Similar abilities are found in birds whose distinctive vocal organs are very different, suggesting that context-specific graded vocal behaviour has evolved independently, as did vocal flexibility and vocal learning. Amongst the primates, gibbons are an especially interesting taxon because, like birds, they produce songs, and may have greater vocal tract control than other non-human primates [64]. More research on gibbon song is needed to describe its complexity and the cognitive factors associated with song production [23,37-39,48,64].

Comparing the vocalisations of non-human primates with human language is of interest because of shared phylogeny. So far, the main difference between human and non-human primates is largely in terms of flexibility in production, but less so in terms of comprehension [65-67]. However, ours and other authors’ research show that non-human primates have the capacity to generate considerable acoustic variability, within the constraints of their basic vocal repertoire, which widens their communicative power considerably.

Conclusion

Lar gibbons reliably produce context-dependent hoo calls in different contexts, including foraging, predator detection, encountering neighbours, and in duet songs. Differences are not just between predator and non-predator contexts, but also within them. We carried out wide-spread acoustic comparisons and found a complex set of subtle spectral parameters that consistently discriminated contexts both within and between sexes. An acoustically unique class were the raptor hoos, which were clearly distinct from all other contexts, presumably designed to cope with avian predators. Our results are in line with other research that has emphasised the importance of graded call systems in communicating external events, such as chimpanzee pant hoots [14], several calls in the Barbary macaque repertoire [68] and chacma baboon grunts [57]. Playback experiments will be needed to determine whether different hoo calls are discriminated by recipients and whether they elicit appropriate responses, as it has been shown for other graded systems (baboons [69,70]; macaques [69,71]; vervet monkeys [72]). We propose that subtle contextual variation in otherwise homologous calls might be a way to increase the expressive power of non-human primate vocalisations where extensive vocal flexibility is not possible.

Methods

Study site and subjects

The study took place at Mo Singto in Khao Yai National Park, Thailand, approximately 130 km NE of Bangkok (101°229E, 14°269 N), and at an elevation of 730-860 m. The study groups consisted of between two and six individuals, typically an adult pair and their offspring, sometimes with more than one adult male. Males and females typically disperse from their natal group at around 10 years of age, about two years after reaching sexual maturity [73]. Lar gibbons are sexually monomorphic and of light or dark pelage colour, which is unrelated to sex or age. Data were collected from thirteen groups with all group members individually known (with the exception of one male from a neighbouring unhabituated group) and long-term social records were available for most individuals [74].

Data collection and equipment

Data were collected between April 2004 and July 2005 and between May and November 2007. Groups were located by listening to their morning duets or by encountering them at their sleeping sites chosen the previous evening. Groups were usually followed from the first encounter in the morning until they had located their evening sleeping tree, the time of which varied greatly depending on the season. During these focal follows, all hoo calls were recorded and their corresponding contexts noted (Figure 3, Table 5). To elicit the rare predator hoos, we presented a series of fake predators, in realistic poses, close to the ground (terrestrial predators: tiger, clouded leopard), 1-2 m above ground on a low branch (arboreal predator: reticulated python) as well as in the canopy (aerial predator: crested serpent eagle). For full methodological details of these experimental presentations, see Clarke et al. [37]. Only hoos emitted by adults (including young adult offspring still residing in their natal group) were included in the analysis to avoid possible age-related acoustic variation seen in other primates [75]. Recordings were made with a Sennheiser directional microphone with windshield (ME66) and a Sony DAT recorder (TCD-D7).

Data analysis

For each context, we extracted the first five hoos of each call bout for measurements. This was based on the assumption that if calls functioned to transmit something about the event encountered by the caller, then this should already be conveyed acoustically in the early parts of an utterance. In some instances, not every hoo of the first five could be measured completely, usually due to poor recording quality or overlap with other sounds. In these cases, the additional calls beyond the first five were included in the analyses. For each context we collected recordings from at least five individuals of each sex. Exceptions include alert, separation and snake hoos (see Table 2).

All calls were transferred to a computer using Cool Edit 2000 and Adobe Audition 3.0 before analyses with PRAAT 5.0.29 using the following settings: Spectrogram settings: window length = 0.05 s; analysis method = Fourier; window shape = Gaussian; number of time steps = 1,000; number of frequency steps = 250; maximum dB/Hz = 100.0; pre-emphasis dB/oct = 6.0; dynamic compression = 0.0; Intensity settings: Averaging method = mean energy; subtract mean pressure checked; Pulse settings: Maximum period factor = 1.3; maximum amplitude factor = 1.6. The viewing window was approximately 0.3 s.

The following six acoustic parameters were extracted from calls based on ECs observations and MJ Owren and CD Linker [76]: duration (s); intensity (dB); peak frequency (Hz), low frequency (Hz), delta frequency (Hz) and inter-call interval (s) (see Figure 4 for illustrations; data available in LabArchives repository [77]).

Figure 3
figure 3

Spectrographic illustration of hoos given in several contexts by males and females.

Figure 4
figure 4

Spectrographic illustration of a single hoo showing how acoustic measurements were taken. Intercall interval was measured as the end of one hoo to the beginning of the next. Intensity was measured using PRAAT’s automated analysis scripts.

Data were analysed and transformed where appropriate to meet the assumptions of parametric tests (delta frequency and intercall interval) using SPSS version20. Linear Mixed Models were used with subject identity as a random factor and included both sex and context as fixed factors. To explore the effects of context on acoustic parameters further, we used additional LMMs with identity as a random factor in pairwise analyses comparing different contexts using Bonferoni corrections for multiple comparisons.

A Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA) was also conducted to determine how the interaction between acoustic variables might predict membership to the different context groups.

Availability of supporting data

The data set supporting the results of this article are available in the LabArchives repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.6070/H49C6VCK

References

  1. Hurford JR. The origins of meaning. In: Oxford University Press. 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Macedonia JM, Evans CS. Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology. 1993;93:177–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Struhsaker TT. Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann SA, editor. Social communication among primates. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1967. p. 281–324.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P. Vervet monkey alarm calls - semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav. 1980;28(NOV):1070–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. How monkeys see the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Gouzoules S, Gouzoules H, Marler P. Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Anim Behav. 1984;32(1):182–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Zuberbühler K. Referential labelling in diana monkeys. Anim Behav. 2000;59:917–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Di Bitetti MS. Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour. 2003;140:565–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K. Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) - evidence from playback experiments. Ethology. 2006;112(4):346–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Crockford C, Boesch C. Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Anim Behav. 2003;66:115–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Curr Biol. 2005;15(19):1779–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. J Comp Psychol. 2005;119(1):67–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? Anim Behav. 2006;72(5):989–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Notman H, Rendall D. Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Anim Behav. 2005;70:177–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Faragó T, Pongrácz P, Range F, Virányi Z, Miklósi A. 'The bone is mine': affective and referrential aspects of dog growls. Anim Behav. 2010;79:917–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Janik VM. Cognitive skills in bottlenose dolphic communication. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(4):157–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Wilson DR, Evans CS. Fowl communicate the size, speed and proximity of avian stimuli through graded structure in referential alarm calls. Anim Behav. 2012;83:535–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Griesser M. Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Current biology :CB. 2008;18:69–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K. Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science. 2005;308(5730):1934–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Zuberbühler K. A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Anim Behav. 2002;63(2):293–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Zuberbühler K. Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc London, Ser B. 2000;267(1444):713–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB. Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2004;271:755–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Clarke E. The vocalisations and anti-predatory behaviourof wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Khao Yai National Park. Thailand. PhD: University of St Andrews; 2010. http://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/bitstream/10023/1688/3/EstherClarkePhDThesis.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  24. Wheeler BC, Fischer J. Functionally referential signals: a promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2012;21:195–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Townsend SW, Manser MB. Functionally referential communication in mammals: the past, present and the future. Ethology. 2013;119:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Fitch WT. The evolution of language: a comparative review. Biol Philos. 2005;20(2–3):193–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Maynard-Smith J. The evolution of alarm calls. Am Nat. 1965;99:59–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB, editors. The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: Univerity of Chicago Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Mccomb K, Semple S. Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett. 2005;1(4):381–5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Vocal communication in chimpanzees. In: Lonsdorf EV, Ross SR, Matsuzawa T, editors. The mind of the chimpanzee. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2010. p. 192–207.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A. Primate communication: Meaning from strings of calls. In: Lowenthal F, Lefebvre L, editors. Language and recursion. New York: Springer; 2014. p. 115–25.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  32. Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A. Convergence and divergence in diana monkey vocalizations. Biol Lett. 2012;8:382–5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Clay Z, Zuberbühler K. Food-associated calling sequences on bonobos. Anim Behav. 2009;77:1387–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Clay Z, Zuberbühler K. Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS One. 2011;6(4):e18786.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mitani JC. Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour. 1985;82:59–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Raemaekers PM, Raemaekers JJ. Long-range vocal interactions between groups of gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behaviour. 1985;95:26–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K. The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon song. PLoS One. 2006;1(1):e73.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Koda H, Nishimura T, Tokuda IT, Oyakawa C, Nihonmatsu T, Masataka N. Soprano singing in gibbons. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2012;149(3):347–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Koda H, Lemasson A, Oyakawa C, Rizaldi, Pamungkas J, Masataka N. Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e71432.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Carpenter CR. A field study in siam of the behavior and social relations of the gibbon (Hylobates lar). Comparative Psychology Monographs. 1940;16(5):1–212.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Ellefson JO. A natural history of white-handed gibbons in the malayan peninsula. Gibbon and siamang. 1974;3:2–134.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM, Haimoff EH. Loud calls of the gibbon (Hylobates lar) - repertoire, organization and context. Behaviour. 1984;91:146–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Leigh SR, Shea BT. Ontogeny and the evolution of adult body size dimorphism in apes. Am J Primatol. 1995;36(1):37–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Reichard UH, Ganpanakngan M, Barelli C. White-handed gibbons of Khao Yai: Social flexibility, complex reproductive strategies, and a slow life history. 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Haimoff EH. The organization of song in the agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis). Folia Primatol. 1984;42(1):42–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Oyakawa C, Koda H, Sugiura H. Acoustic features contributing to the individuality of wild agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis agilis) songs. Am J Primatol. 2007;69(7):777–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Mitani JC. Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour. 1985;92:59–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K. The anti-predator behaviour of wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012;66:85–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Jones KJ, Hill WL. Auditory perception of hawks and owls for passerine alarm calls. Ethology. 2001;107(8):717–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Ferguson-Lees J, Christie DA. Raptors of the world. London: Helm; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Beason RC. What can birds hear? In: USDA National Wildlife Research left - Staff Publications. 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Zuberbühler K. Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild diana monkeys. Anim Behav. 2000;59:209–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R. The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology. 1999;105(6):477–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Taylor AM, Reby D. The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. J Zool. 2010;280:221–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Smith RJ, Jungers WL. Body mass in comparative primatology. J Hum Evol. 1997;32(6):523–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Barelli C, Mundry R, Heistermann M, Hammerschmidt K: Cues to androgens and quality in male gibbon songs. PLoS One 2013, 8(12):e82748.

  57. Owren MJ, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. The acoustic features of vowel-like grunt calls in chacma baboons (Papio cyncephalus ursinus): Implications for production processes and functions. J Acoust Soc Am. 1997;101(5):2951–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Scott-Phillips T. Animal communication: Insights from linguistic pragmatics. Anim Behav. 2010;79:e1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Rendall D, Owren M, Ryan MJ. What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav. 2009;78:233–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbuhler K, Slocombe KE. Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Anim Behav. 2013;86(5):955–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Simonyan K. The laryngeal motor cortex: Its organization and connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;28:15–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Zuberbühler K: Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evol Biol 2011, 11:362.

  63. Lewis MP, Simons GF, Fennig CD, editors. Ethnologue: Languages of the world. Seventeenthth ed. Dallas, Texas: SIL International; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Koda H, Oyakawa C, Kato A, Masataka N. Experimental evidence for the volitional control of vocal production in an immature gibbon. Behaviour. 2007;144:681–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Calcagno JM, Fuentes A. What makes us human? answers from evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2012;21:182–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Burling R. The talking ape. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang. 2010;115(1):92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J. The vocal repertoire of Barbary macaques: a quantitative analysis of a graded system. Ethology. 1998;104:203–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Fischer J, Metz M, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Anim Behav. 2001;61:925–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Rendall D, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Owren MJ. The meaning and function of grunt variants in baboons. Anim Behav. 1999;57:583–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Fischer J. Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types. Anim Behav. 1998;55:799–807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. How vervet monkeys perceive their grunts: field playback experiments. Anim Behav. 1982;30(3):739–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JJ. Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar). BES. 1998;42:329–39.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Savini T, Boesch C, Reichard UH. Varying ecological quality influences the probability of polyandry in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). Biotropica. 2009;41:503–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Ey E, Hammerschmidt K, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J. Age- and sex-related variations in clear calls of papio ursinus. Int J Primatol. 2007;28:947–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Owren MJ, Linker CD. Some analysis techniques that may be useful to acoustic primatologists. In: Zimmermann E, Newman JD, Jürgens U, editors. Current topics in primate vocal communication. NY: Plenum; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Clarke E, Reichard, UH, Zuberbühler, K: Acoustic measures of gibbon (Hylobates lar) hoo calls in different contexts. LabArchives 2015, http://dx.doi.org/10.6070/H49C6VCK.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the following for research assistance during data collection: Brendon Snyder, Anna Wilkinson, Annette Jeneson, Melanie Beuerlein, Nicola Uhde, David Costa-Schellenberger and Nathan Oesch. Thanks also goes to S. Homros (Jimmy), C. Mungpoonklang (Adt), S. Seeboon (Tiang), and S. Sornchaipoom (Dtai), who helped tracking the gibbon groups. Our gratitude further goes to the National Research Council, the National Park Division, the Wildlife and Plant Conservation Department, and the Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment, and their superintendent of Khao Yai National Park, Mr. Khun Prawat Vohandee, for permission to conduct research at Khao Yai. In addition, thanks go to Stefano Vaglio, Joanna Setchell, Russell Hill, Robert Seyfarth and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council PhD studentship awarded to EC.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Esther Clarke.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

EC and KZ conceived of and designed the study, EC collected and analysed data, UHR provided access to long-term demographic records, trained field assistants, institutional support and invaluable advice. All three authors helped to draft the manuscript and have read and approved the final version.

Authors’ information

EC is a Junior Research Fellow in the Evolutionary Anthropology Research Group at Durham University, UK. UHR is an Associate Professor at the Department of Anthropology and left for Ecology, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, U.S.A. KZ is a Professor at the Department of Comparative Cognition, Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland and also a Professor at the School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews KY16 9JP, UK.

Rights and permissions

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Clarke, E., Reichard, U.H. & Zuberbühler, K. Context-specific close-range “hoo” calls in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar). BMC Evol Biol 15, 56 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0332-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0332-2

Keywords