Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Male mate choice scales female ornament allometry in a cichlid fish

Abstract

Background

Studies addressing the adaptive significance of female ornamentation have gained ground recently. However, the expression of female ornaments in relation to body size, known as trait allometry, still remains unexplored. Here, we investigated the allometry of a conspicuous female ornament in Pelvicachromis taeniatus, a biparental cichlid that shows mutual mate choice and ornamentation. Females feature an eye-catching pelvic fin greatly differing from that of males.

Results

We show that allometry of the female pelvic fin is scaled more positively in comparison to other fins. The pelvic fin exhibits isometry, whereas the other fins (except the caudal fin) show negative allometry. The size of the pelvic fin might be exaggerated by male choice because males prefer female stimuli that show a larger extension of the trait. Female pelvic fin size is correlated with individual condition, suggesting that males can assess direct and indirect benefits.

Conclusions

The absence of positive ornament allometry might be a result of sexual selection constricted by natural selection: fins are related to locomotion and thus may be subject to viability selection. Our study provides evidence that male mate choice might scale the expression of a female sexual ornament, and therefore has implications for the understanding of the relationship of female sexual traits with body size in species with conventional sex-roles.

Background

Male traits that have evolved under sexual selection, e.g. ornaments to attract potential mates, and their exaggeration have been intensely investigated in many taxa [1, 2]. However, in many species females show conspicuous ornaments as well [3]. Although female ornamentation has long been considered as non-adaptive, solely being a result of a genetic correlation to male ornaments [4], theoretical work and empirical studies suggest that trait expression in females could be promoted by male mate choice in species in which paternal investment and variance in female quality are high [57]. Studies addressing the adaptive significance of sexual selection in females have gained ground recently [3, 810]. Nevertheless, evidence that male choice scales the exaggeration of a female trait is still lacking in many taxa.

Individual shape, such as morphology of ornaments or other sexual traits, is defined by the dimensions of the respective body part in relation to body size, with the scale relating trait size to body size known as allometry [11, 12]. Allometric relationships are usually calculated by log-transformed allometric linear equations, Y = aXb, b representing the allometric slope and X and Y representing the indices of body size and trait size, respectively. Allometric relationships are classified in (a) isometry (b = 1), where the ratio of trait:body size remains constant, (b) negative allometry (b < 1), where larger individuals have relatively smaller traits, and (c) positive allometry (b > 1), where larger individuals show relatively larger traits. Allometry is suspected to evolve by three main forces: (a) evolutionary constraints, (b) natural selection, and (c) sexual selection [13, 14]. Traditionally, traits under directional sexual selection are expected to show positive allometry [15]. Striking examples for positive allometry are extraordinarily exaggerated structures, such as antlers of the Irish elk, Megaloceros giganteos [16] or the horns of different beetles species [e.g. [15, 17, 18]]. In contrast to the traditional view, Bonduriansky [12, 14] suggested that both sexual and natural selection may produce a range of allometric relationships depending on net selection on body size and traits. For example, dedicated secondary sexual traits may indeed show positive allometry, but sexually selected modifications to structures with important viability-related functions may exhibit isometric or even negative allometric scaling due to conflicting sexual and viability selection. Consequently, the absence of positive allometry does not necessarily imply that a trait is not affected by sexual selection.

Allometric relationships of male traits and their homologous expressions in females have been investigated in a wide range of species [e.g. [14] and citations therein, [19]]. However, this aspect is less well examined in species in which females show distinct sexual traits that are not expressed by males, for example many (biparental) fish species. Whereas empirical studies now repeatedly have shown that female ornamentation is subject to male sexual selection even in species with conventional sex-roles [e.g. [2022]] knowledge about the impact of male choice on female trait allometry and the extent to which female ornamentation signals benefits to males and is still scarce. Lebas et al. [23] provided one of the few examples that allometry of a female ornament in the dance fly, Rhamphomyia tarsata, might signal fecundity to males.

In general, surprisingly few studies deal with trait allometry in fishes but see [24], although in many fishes the sexes show exaggerated ornaments like elongated, colorful fins, which are assumed to have resulted from sexual selection [1]. For example in swordtail fish, Xiphophorus helleri, females prefer males that show a sword-shaped fin [25]. In cichlids, the role of either male or female fin size in mate choice is largely unknown. Moreover, potential sexual ornaments in female cichlids, such as the size of the female pelvic fin, are solely considered to be important for comfort movements [e.g. [26]] or egg placement [27], and thus assumed to be not socially significant.

In the present study we examined the allometry and sexual selection of a female sexual trait in the biparental, socially monogamous cichlid fish, Pelvicachromis taeniatus, in which the female pelvic fin is a conspicuous sexual ornament. Both sexes of P. taeniatus show such ornaments, as well as other sexual traits. Females develop exceedingly large pelvic fins, which also differ from male fins in shape and color: females exhibit a triangular fan-shaped fin, whereas males develop a long thread-shaped fin. During courtship, both sexes present their nuptially colored ventral body region by arching it towards the partner, while intensely quivering the whole body. Simultaneously, females fan out their violet pelvic fin, thereby enlarging their violet ventral nuptial projection area, suggesting that the fin is actively used during mate choice. The female pelvic fin might function as an exaggeration of the female's ventral nuptial coloration area, which acts as a quality signal in intersexual communication (SAB, TCMB, HK, TT unpublished data).

Recent studies have shown that both sexes of P. taeniatus are choosy during mate choice: close kin are preferred over non-kin [28, 29] and larger mating partners over smaller ones [30]. Furthermore, females of P. taeniatus prefer yellow nuptially colored males over dull males [31], and males prefer females that show a larger extent of violet nuptial coloration. Thus, P. taeniatus seems to be a well-suited model system concerning male choice and its impact on female trait allometry.

First, we investigated the allometric relationship of the pelvic in comparison to the other fins, i.e. anal, caudal, dorsal, and pectoral fin. Estimates of body and fin sizes were based on measurements of bony structures. For this purpose, X-ray images of females (see Figure 1) that greatly differed in body size were analyzed. Second, we experimentally tested male preferences for females that showed larger or smaller pelvic fin size in order to test whether it is currently influencing male mate choice. To achieve this we conducted a series of mate choice experiments using computer animations of females that artificially differed in the size of the pelvic fin. A striking advantage of computer-manipulated stimuli is a high degree of standardization between the stimuli, thus minimizing the effects of confounding variables like rapid changes in coloration or different responses in stimuli fish [3133]. Although computer systems and their visual displays are tailored to human vision they have been shown to be an appropriate method to investigate preferences in our study species concerning the perception of movement, shape and coloration [31].

Figure 1
figure 1

Sample of an X-ray image. Example of an X-Ray image of a female P. taeniatus. One of the paired pelvic fins was dissected in order to avoid errors in measurements by overlap in X-Rays.

Results

Fin allometry

The size of each fin group was highly significantly related to body size (Pearson and Spearman rank correlations: all r > 0.48, all p < 0.001). However, allometry significantly differed between fin groups (full model lrt: interaction between fin groups and body size, χ2 = 17.97, p = 0.0012). Post-hoc tests revealed that the pelvic fin differed significantly in allometry from the anal fin (lrt: χ2 = 6.24, p = 0.012; Figure 2a), and highly significantly from the dorsal fin (lrt: χ2 = 12.69, p < 0.001; Figure 2c). Pelvic fin's allometry tended to be different from that of the pectoral fin (lrt: χ2 = 3.76, p = 0.052; Figure 2b), however, was not significantly different from that of the caudal fin (lrt: χ2 = 0.72, p = 0.39; Figure 2d). The size of the pelvic fin was significantly related to individual body condition (Spearman rank correlation: n = 79, r = 0.31, p = 0.004).

Figure 2
figure 2

Differences between allometric slopes of fins. Pairwise comparisons of the allometric slopes between the pelvic fin (crosses, solid lines) and other fins (dots, dotted lines): a) anal fin, b) pectoral fin, c) dorsal fin, d) caudal fin.

The pelvic and the caudal fin were both isometric because their allometric slopes did not significantly differ from 1 (Table 1). In contrast, the relationship between fin size and body size was negatively allometric for of the anal, dorsal and pectoral fin (Table 1) because their slopes were significantly smaller than 1.

Table 1 Allometric slope of fins

Male mate choice experiment

Males significantly preferred the female stimulus showing a larger pelvic fin (lrt: n = 18, df = 1, χ2 = 5.33, p = 0.02; Figure 3). Neither body size of males (lrt: n = 18, df = 1, χ2 = 1.18, p = 0.28) nor differences in stimulus size (lrt: n = 18, df = 1, χ2 = 0.15, p = 0.93) had any significant effects.

Figure 3
figure 3

Males preferences. Male preferences for female stimuli that presented different sizes of the pelvic fin (100% vs. 50%, 50% vs. 0% and 100% vs. 0% pelvic fin size). The amount of time (means + S.D.) males spent in the association zones in three experimental treatments with the female stimulus that either showed a larger or a smaller pelvic fin size is shown. A linear mixed effect model was fitted (see the text for details). * p < 0.05.

Discussion and Conclusion

Our study is one of the first to investigate the allometry of a conspicuous female ornament, in this case the size of the female pelvic fin in a cichlid fish. The results demonstrate that the allometry of the female sexual ornament (pelvic fin) is scaled more positively than that of the other fins. Male preference for a larger female pelvic fin size in P. taeniatus suggests that sexual selection in females might have influenced female trait allometry.

Fin allometry

The pelvic and caudal fins show isometry in relation to body size, whereas the anal, dorsal and pectoral fins show negative allometry: females exhibit a constant ratio of pelvic or caudal fin size to body size, whereas the other fins are relatively smaller in larger females. Furthermore, significant differences between the slopes of log-log regressions of the female fins support differences in their allometric relationships. The strongest differences in slopes occur between the pelvic fin in comparison to the anal, dorsal, and to a lesser extent to the pectoral fin. The caudal fin showed a similar relative growth in relation to body size. Thus, the size of the pelvic and caudal fin is more positively related to body size than the other fins. These results imply that the female pelvic, as well as the female caudal fin, underlie other selective pressures than the other fins. The result of the male choice experiment suggests that the female pelvic fin size might have been exaggerated by male choice.

Negative allometric relationships (b < 1) are usually thought to be characteristic for non-sexually selected traits [34, 35], whereas sexually selected traits should exhibit positive allometry (b > 1). Thus our finding of an isometric relationship in a sexually selected trait contradicts these traditional views on the effect of sexual selection on trait allometry [e.g. [15]]. However, Bonduriansky and Day [12] specify a more complex relationship between selection on body size and traits by incorporating viability selection. Natural and sexual selection could have synergistic effects on the evolution of traits, thus sexually selected traits may be scaled into isometry or even negative allometry. Recent studies have demonstrated that sexually selected characters do not need to exhibit positive allometry [36, 37]. Furthermore, House and Simmons [38] suggested that the amount of variation in traits under directional sexual selection may be limited by natural selection. Such constraints might explain the absence of positive allometry in a trait that is related to locomotion or other viability related functions. In P. taeniatus natural selection might constrain the pelvic fin size because oversized pelvic fins may lower the predator escape response of this fish. Thus, the isometric growth might be a result of sexual selection constrained by natural selection acting on the size of the pelvic fin.

The caudal fin allometry was not significantly different from that of the pelvic fin. The selective pressures acting on caudal fin size were not object of the present study. In other fishes a range from negative to positive allometric relationships of the caudal fin size has been reported [3941]. Recent work in other fish species suggests that natural selection, e.g. predation, influences caudal fin morphology [4245]. For example, in western mosquitofish, Gambusia affinis, fish originating from populations which are subject to high levels of predation exhibit a larger caudal fin than those from predator-free populations, resulting in greater maximum burst-swimming [43]. Nevertheless, sexual selection cannot be ruled out as a selective force that might scale the size of the caudal fin as well like in guppies, Poecilia reticulata [46]. Although the disentanglement of natural and sexual selection acting on different fins is difficult, future studies could address whether changes in allometry between fins might appear, for example under different environmental conditions. Artificial selection experiments or measurements of phenotypic plasticity could address whether environmental conditions, such as predation regime or water-flow speed, might have different impacts on fin allometry. On the other hand, mate choice experiments could elucidate the role of sexual selection acting on the size and allometry of different fins.

Male mate choice experiment

Males associated more often with female stimuli showing a larger pelvic fin size. Why are male P. taeniatus sensitive to female pelvic fin size? Female ornaments could act as indicators of female quality [47]. Males may benefit from being choosy through (a) direct benefits, for example deriving from female fertility, fecundity or the amount of maternal care, or (b) from indirect, genetic benefits for their offspring [2], deriving from enhanced viability or parasite resistance or enhanced attractiveness of daughters [48, 49]. Our results show that pelvic fin size of female P. taeniatus is positively related to body condition, thus suggesting that the size of the female pelvic fin reveals individual quality: females with high body condition may show a decreased mortality risk through disease or starvation during brood care [50], and sire daughters that are more attractive to males of the following generation. Thus, male P. taeniatus may get both direct as well as indirect benefits when choosing females that show pronounced expression of the quality signal.

The pelvic fins are colored similar to the ventral violet nuptial belly coloration of a female. The extent of the violet ventral coloration is associated with female quality revealing female fecundity, readiness to spawn, maternal quality and offspring-survival (SAB, TCMB, HK, TT unpublished data). Consequently, the pelvic fin could honestly enhance the transfer of quality information about an individual, and males could expect direct fitness benefits when choosing a female that shows a larger pelvic fin. However, the trait might be used to exploit male preferences, especially when the expression of the trait may imply low costs. Nevertheless, the positive correlation between pelvic fin size and individual condition contradicts the exploitation hypothesis, but future studies need to address whether the pelvic fin as well as its coloration honestly signals female quality and the heritability of the trait.

In addition to male choice, intrasexual female-female competition may be important for the evolution of females' pelvic fin size. In P. taeniatus, like in many other cichlids [26], females show sequential aggressive behavioral patterns towards other conspecific females. Before a fight escalates females form their body into an S-shape to threaten a potential rival. Here, the female pelvic fin is fanned out to enlarge the lateral projection area. Hence, a larger pelvic fin size might imply advantages during female-female competition, for example when the decision whether to attack or escape from a rival is estimated by a larger lateral display area. Thus, the size of the pelvic may evolve by both inter- and intrasexual selection.

Female showiness expressed by morphological traits, such as conspicuous ornaments, is widespread in many taxa. Moreover, recent research suggests that male choice occurs far more often than expected [51]. Our study is, to our knowledge, the first to show that male choice might scale the allometry of a female sexual trait, and therefore has implications for the understanding of the scaling relationship of female traits with body size.

Methods

Fin allometry

Experimental animals

Animals used for measurements were adult lab-bred fish raised under standardized laboratory conditions. The parents of the fish originated from the Moliwe river in Cameroon (04°04'N/09°16'E), West Africa. The fish were a mixed stock from different cohorts, thus varying substantially in body size. Prior to measurements, the fish were kept in eight different aquaria (50 × 30 × 30 cm). The water was tempered at 25°C and a 12:12 h (light:dark) cycle was provided to resemble natural conditions. Fish were fed daily with a mixture of frozen Chironomus larvae and Artemia spp. The measurements were conducted between April 20th, 2009 and May 25th, 2009. Altogether, 79 females were measured for the allometric examinations. The study conforms to the legal requirements of Germany for the use of animals in research.

Fin measurements

Fish were caught in random order from the tanks. In order to avoid damage of the fins, each female was carefully caught with a hand net, and its body mass was measured immediately. However, damages of fin tissue resulting from intra-specific conflicts can affect the estimate of fin size based on fin area. Therefore, we measured fin size as the length of fin rays. Fin ray length was highly significantly related to fin area (see Table 2). Fin area was calculated by analyzing a standardized digital photograph of each fish (including a size scale (mm2) in the scene) that was taken immediately before pelvic fin dissection. The fins were fanned out orthogonal to the body and fixed with a needle. The fin area (in mm2) was then estimated by counting the number of pixels of each fin and setting the value in relation to the number of pixels that made up the size standard (Sigma Scan pro 5).

Table 2 Relationship between fin ray length and fin area

In order to avoid damage to the fins fish were shock-frozen by placing them into saltwater that was cooled down to a temperature of -14°C. After taking the photograph the pelvic fin was dissected. The fish body and the dissected fin were preserved in ethanol (70%). The fish were X-rayed together with their dissected pelvic fin in a digital X-ray device (Faxitron Digital Specimen Radiography System LX-60) with an integrated digital camera (Figure 1). A size scale was installed during all X-rays.

Fin ray length was taken as proxy for fin size, measured to the nearest micrometer from digital X-ray-images using the software ImageJ (version 1.41; [52]). Pelvic fin length was measured as length of the pelvic spine from the X-rays of the dissected pelvic fin. In case of the anal fin, length of the 3rd (the longest) anal spine was quantified. Likewise, length of the 17th (last and longest) dorsal spine was measured. Caudal fin length was estimated by calculating a mean value from the four middle caudal fin rays (the longest caudal rays in P. taeniatus). Total body size was measured as standard length from the upper jaw symphysis to the end of the hypural plate, based on the X-ray-images. Pectoral fin ray length was measured with a digital calliper directly from the preserved fish, measuring the longest fin ray. Individual body condition was calculated as a function of body mass and standard length ((100 × mass)/standard length3 [53]).

Statistical analysis

Parametric Pearson correlation tests were applied when data were normally distributed according to Shapiro Wilk tests, otherwise non-parametric Spearman rank correlation tests were used. All metric variables in models were log transformed and were graphically inspected for normality by using normal quantile plots of the log regressions. For analysis we used R 2.9.1 software package [54]. Linear mixed effect models ("lme", package "nlme" [55]) were fitted to measure differences in allometry between fin groups. Fin size was used as dependent variable, fin type as fixed factor and female body size as covariate. Female identity was entered as a random factor to account for the fact that the fins were not independent from each other. A significant interaction between fin groups and body size would reveal that the fins differ in their allometry. Thus, we first fitted a full model including all fin groups and tested the interaction with body size. Second, post-hoc pairwise comparisons between the pelvic fin and the other fin types concerning their allometric relationships were conducted by testing whether the slopes were significantly different from each other. Reported p-values refer to the increase in deviance in model fit when the respective variable was removed (likelihood-ratio-tests ("lrt")).

To test whether the relationship between the size of each fin and body size were isometric, negatively or positively allometric, we used a procedure analogous to Student's t-test, testing whether the allometric slopes were significantly different from one. Given p-values are two-tailed throughout. P-values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Male mate choice experiment

Experimental animals

All individuals of P. taeniatus were bred and maintained under standardized laboratory conditions. The parents of the test subjects originated from the Moliwe river in Cameroon (04°04'N/09°16'E), West Africa. All 18 test individuals were derived from the F1 generation sired from 18 unrelated pairs and were raised in mixed-sex family tanks (80 × 30 × 30 cm). The tanks were surrounded with opaque plastic sheets to avoid visual contact to other aquaria. Test fish were 1-2 years old and reproductively active. The water temperature was kept at 25 ± 1°C and natural light conditions were given (light/dark 12/12 h). Nutrition was provided once a day with a mixture of frozen Chironomus spp. and Artemia spp.

Preparation of artificial stimuli and experimental design

Artificial stimuli like computer animations provide a high degree of experimental standardization. However, computer displays are tailored to human vision and do not emit wavelengths such as ultraviolet or polarized light [32, 56], which may play a role in the vision of shallow cichlid species [57]. Hence, it might be possible that artificial computer animations are perceived differently from the way real fishes would appear in the natural environment. However, previous studies have shown that P. taeniatus reliably perceives computer stimuli concerning movement, body shape and coloration displayed by a cathode-ray-tube monitor [30, 31].

A digital photograph (Olympus Camedia Widezoom 5060) of a nuptially colored female was taken to obtain source data for a two-dimensional fish model. Images were saved in RAW-format to avoid the loss of coloration data due to algorithmic compression. They were white-balanced during import to Adobe Photoshop CS3. To achieve animations of the models we used "The GIMP 2.20 with animation package". A grey background image (1024 × 400 px) was created (RGB: 238, 238, 238) including a plant as a reference object in the middle of the background. Each animation consisted of 30 frames per second, which is an established method to present artificial stimuli to our test species [32]. Each stimulus moved a horizontal pathway from one side of the monitor to the other for a period of 15 seconds, including a two second stop in the middle. After that, the stimulus recurred horizontally and moved back in the same time frame. We created three different experimental treatments showing female stimuli differing in pelvic fin size (100% vs. 50%, 50% vs. 0% and 100% vs. 0% pelvic fin size). Each treatment was conducted twice with stimuli shown on switched monitor sides. Thus, each male received six experimental trials, which were conducted in random order.

Experiments were conducted between January 12th, 2009 and February 20th, 2009. Males were randomly chosen and individually isolated in separate tanks (30 × 20 × 20 cm). The mating readiness of each test fish was determined visually on the basis of the ventral coloration as well as the display of courtship behavior in the family tanks [28, 29]. The isolation tanks were surrounded by print-outs of the animation's background at the broad sides and Styrofoam at the longer sides, thus ensuring that fish did not interact with other isolated individuals and were able to habituate to the background and the reference object. In each habitat aquarium a breeding cave was installed. All other conditions were similar to those of the mixed-sex tanks. Test fish were transported to the experimental set-up in their isolation tank, thus reducing stress by leaving the fish in its familiar habitat.

The set-up was illuminated by a fluorescent tube (37 W) installed one meter above the middle of the tank. Additionally, white Styrofoam surrounded the set-up. The habitat aquarium containing the test fish was placed between two CRT monitors of the same model (EIZO Flex Scan F520, 85 Hz, connected to a Matrox G550 PCIe graphic board). An association zone of 5 cm in front of each monitor was marked on the white Styrofoam under the tank creating a 20 cm neutral zone in between.

During an acclimatization period of 15 minutes both screens showed the background. After acclimatization, the stimuli appeared simultaneously on both monitors [58]. Fish behavior was recorded using a webcam. After all experimental treatments the standard length of the test fish was measured. A naïve observer analyzed the video recordings. Mating preferences were measured as association time near a stimulus of the opposite sex, which reliably predicts mating decisions in P. taeniatus [28]. The time spent in each association zone was calculated over a period of two minutes after the fish had first visited an association zone. For each test fish, we averaged the time spent in front of each stimulus in the first and the second trial of each treatment, thus excluding possible side biases.

Statistical analysis

Data were tested for normality with Shapiro Wilk tests and analyzed by fitting a linear mixed effect model ("lme", package: "nlme" [55]), with the time in front of the stimuli as dependent and stimulus type (larger or smaller pelvic fin) as explanatory variable, and male identity as random factor. Furthermore, body size and treatment (100% vs. 50%, 50% vs. 0% and 100% vs. 0% pelvic fin size) were included into the model to reveal whether these factors have an impact on male choice. Significant differences between treatments would suggest that larger differences between stimuli translate into stronger male preference.

References

  1. Darwin C: On the descent of man and selection in relation to sex. 1871, London: John Murray

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  2. Andersson M: Sexual selection. 1994, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  3. Amundsen T: Why are female birds ornamented?. Trends Ecol Evol. 2000, 15: 149-155. 10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01800-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Lande R: Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection and adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution. 1980, 34: 292-305. 10.2307/2407393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kokko H, Johnstone RA: Why is mutual mate choice not the norm? Operational sex ratios, sex roles and the evolution of sexually dimorphic and monomorphic signalling. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 2002, 357: 319-330. 10.1098/rstb.2001.0926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Parker GA: Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 2006, 361: 235-259. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1785.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Clutton-Brock TH, Vincent ACJ: Sexual selection and the potential reproductive rates of males and females. Nature. 1991, 351: 58-60. 10.1038/351058a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Amundsen T, Forsgren E: Male mate choice selects for female coloration in a fish. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001, 98: 13155-13160. 10.1073/pnas.211439298.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kraaijeveld K, Kraaijeveld-Smit FJL, Komdeur J: The evolution of mutual ornamentation. Anim Behav. 2007, 74: 657-677. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hill GE: Male mate choice and the evolution of female plumage coloration in the house finch. Evolution. 1993, 47: 1515-1525. 10.2307/2410164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gould SJ: Allometry and size in ontogeny and phylogeny. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 1966, 41: 587-638. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1966.tb01624.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bonduriansky R, Day T: The evolution of static allometry in sexually selected traits. Evolution. 2003, 57: 2450-2458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Dale J, Dunn PO, Figuerola J, Lislevand T, Szekely T, Whittingham LA: Sexual selection explains Rensch's rule of allometry for sexual size dimorphism. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2007, 274: 2971-2979. 10.1098/rspb.2007.1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Bonduriansky R: Sexual selection and allometry: a critical reappraisal of the evidence and ideas. Evolution. 2007, 61: 838-849. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00081.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kodric-Brown A, Sibly RM, Brown JH: The allometry of ornaments and weapons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006, 103: 8733-8738. 10.1073/pnas.0602994103.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gould SJ: Positive allometry of antlers in the "Irish elk", Megaloceros giganteus. Nature. 1973, 244: 375-376. 10.1038/244375a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Emlen DJ: Artificial selection on horn length body size allometry in the horned beetle Onthophagus acuminatus (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae). Evolution. 1996, 50: 1219-1230. 10.2307/2410662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Eberhard WG: Beetle horn dimorphism - making the best of a bad lot. Am Nat. 1982, 119: 420-426. 10.1086/283920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Cuervo JJ, Møller AP: Components of phenotypic variation in avian ornamental and non-ornamental feathers. Evol Ecol. 2001, 15: 53-72. 10.1023/A:1011913804309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Amundsen T, Forsgren E, Hansen LTT: On the function of female ornaments: male bluethroats prefer colourful females. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1997, 264: 1579-1586. 10.1098/rspb.1997.0220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Griggio M, Valera F, Casas A, Pilastro A: Males prefer ornamented females: a field experiment of male choice in the rock sparrow. Anim Behav. 2005, 69: 1243-1250. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.10.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Griggio M, Devigili A, Hoi H, Pilastro A: Female ornamentation and directional male mate preference in the rock sparrow. Behav Ecol. 2009, 20: 1072-1078. 10.1093/beheco/arp099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. LeBas NR, Hockham LR, Ritchie MG: Nonlinear and correlational sexual selection on 'honest' female ornamentation. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2003, 270: 2159-2165. 10.1098/rspb.2003.2482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Jennions MD, Kelly CD: Geographical variation in male genitalia in Brachyphaphis episcopi (Poeciliidae): is it sexually or naturally selected?. Oikos. 2002, 97: 79-86. 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2002.970108.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rosenthal GG, Evans CS: Female preference for swords in Xiphophorus helleri reflects a bias for large apparent size. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998, 95: 4431-4436. 10.1073/pnas.95.8.4431.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Barlow GW: The cichlid fishes - Nature's grand experiment in evolution. 2002, Cambridge: Perseus

    Google Scholar 

  27. Ostrander GK, Ward JA: The function of the pelvic fins during courtship and spawning in the orange chromide, Etroplus maculatus. Environ Biol Fishes. 1985, 13: 203-210. 10.1007/BF00000932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Thünken T, Bakker TCM, Baldauf SA, Kullmann H: Active inbreeding in a cichlid fish and its adaptive significance. Curr Biol. 2007, 17: 225-229. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Thünken T, Bakker TCM, Baldauf SA, Kullmann H: Direct familiarity does not alter mating preferences for sisters in male Pelvicachromis taeniatus (Cichlidae). Ethology. 2007, 113: 1107-1112. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01422.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Baldauf SA, Kullmann H, Schroth SH, Thünken T, Bakker TCM: You can't always get what you want: size assortative mating by mutual mate choice as a resolution of sexual conflict. BMC Evol Biol. 2009, 9: 129-10.1186/1471-2148-9-129.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Baldauf SA, Kullmann H, Winter S, Thünken T, Bakker TCM: Computer animation as a tool to study preferences in the cichlid Pelvicachromis taeniatus. J Fish Biol. 2009, 75: 738-746. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02347.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Baldauf SA, Kullmann H, Bakker TCM: Technical restrictions of computer-manipulated visual stimuli and display units for studying animal behaviour. Ethology. 2008, 114: 737-751. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01520.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Künzler R, Bakker TCM: Computer animations as a tool in the study of mating preferences. Behaviour. 1998, 135: 1137-1159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Eberhard WG: Natural history and behavior of Chymomyza mycopelates and C-exophthalma (Diptera: Drosophilidae), and allometry of structures used as signals, weapons, and spore collectors. Can Entomol. 2002, 134: 667-687. 10.4039/Ent134667-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Green AJ: The scaling and selection of sexually dimorphic characters: an example using the Marbled Teal. J Avian Biol. 2000, 31: 345-350. 10.1034/j.1600-048X.2000.310310.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bonduriansky R: Convergent evolution of sexual shape dimorphism in diptera. J Morphol. 2006, 267: 602-611. 10.1002/jmor.10426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. van Lieshout E, Elgar MA: Armament under direct sexual selection does not exhibit positive allometry in an earwig. Behav Ecol. 2009, 20: 258-264. 10.1093/beheco/arp013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. House CM, Simmons LW: Genital morphology and fertilization success in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: an example of sexually selected male genitalia. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2003, 270: 447-455. 10.1098/rspb.2002.2266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Ellis T, Hoyle I, Oidtmann B, Turnbull JF, Jacklin TE, Knowles TG: Further development of the "Fin Index" method for quantifying fin erosion in rainbow trout. Aquaculture. 2009, 289: 283-288. 10.1016/j.aquaculture.2009.01.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Lingham-Soliar T: Caudal fin allometry in the white shark Carcharodon carcharias: implications for locomotory performance and ecology. Naturwissenschaften. 2005, 92: 231-236. 10.1007/s00114-005-0614-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Maie T, Schoenfuss HL, Blob RW: Ontogenetic scaling of body proportions in waterfall-climbing gobiid fishes from Hawai'i and Dominica: implications for locomotor function. Copeia. 2007, 3: 755-764. 10.1643/0045-8511(2007)2007[755:OSOBPI]2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Hankison SJ, Childress MJ, Schmitter-Soto JJ, Ptacek MB: Morphological divergence within and between the Mexican sailfin mollies, Poecilia velifera and Poecilia petenensis. J Fish Biol. 2006, 68: 1610-1630. 10.1111/j.0022-1112.2006.001051.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Langerhans RB, Layman CA, Shokrollahi AM, DeWitt TJ: Predator-driven phenotypic diversification in Gambusia affinis. Evolution. 2004, 58: 2305-2318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Leavy TR, Bonner TH: Relationships among swimming ability, current velocity association, and morphology for freshwater lotic fishes. North Am J Fish Manage. 2009, 29: 72-83. 10.1577/M07-040.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Plaut I: Effects of fin size on swimming performance, swimming behaviour and routine activity of zebrafish Danio rerio. J Exp Biol. 2000, 203: 813-820.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Bischoff RJ, Gould JL, Rubenstein DI: Tail size and female choice in the guppy (Poecilia-reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1985, 17: 253-255. 10.1007/BF00300143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Zahavi A: Mate selection - a selection for a handicap. J Theor Biol. 1975, 53: 205-214. 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Fisher RA: The genetical theory of natural selection. 1930, Oxford: Clarendon Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  49. Hamilton WD, Zuk M: Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites?. Science. 1982, 218: 384-387. 10.1126/science.7123238.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Smith C, Wootton RJ: The costs of parental care in teleost fishes. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 1995, 5: 7-22. 10.1007/BF01103363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Hooper PL, Miller GF: Mutual mate choice can drive costly signaling even under perfect monogamy. Adapt Behav. 2008, 16: 53-70. 10.1177/1059712307087283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Rasband WS: ImageJ. Maryland, USA: U. S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, 1997-

  53. Bolger T, Connolly PL: The selection of suitable indices for the measurements and analysis of fish condition. J Fish Biol. 1989, 34: 171-182. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1989.tb03300.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. R-Development-Core-Team: R: a language and environment for statistical computing. 2009, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria

    Google Scholar 

  55. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R-Core-team: nlme: linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-92. 2009

    Google Scholar 

  56. D'Eath RB: Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments?. Biol Rev. 1998, 73 (3): 267-292. 10.1017/S0006323198005179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Seehausen O, Terai Y, Magalhaes IS, Carleton KL, Mrosso HDJ, Miyagi R, van der Sluijs I, Schneider MV, Maan ME, Tachida H, et al: Speciation through sensory drive in cichlid fish. Nature. 2008, 455: 620-623. 10.1038/nature07285.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. MacLaren RD, Rowland WJ: Differences in female preference for male body size in Poecilia latipinna using simultaneous versus sequential stimulus presentation designs. Behaviour. 2006, 143: 273-292. 10.1163/156853906775897897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank T. Ottenheym and D. Meuthen for assistance during experiments. L. Engqvist is acknowledged for statistical support. M. Hiermes is acknowledged for improving our English. This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) (BA 2885/2-3).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastian A Baldauf.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

SAB participated in the design of the study, collected data, performed statistical analyses and wrote the paper. TCMB, FH and HK participated in the design of the study and the writing of the paper. TT participated in the design of the study, statistical analysis and the writing of the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baldauf, S.A., Bakker, T.C., Herder, F. et al. Male mate choice scales female ornament allometry in a cichlid fish. BMC Evol Biol 10, 301 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-10-301

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-10-301

Keywords