Skip to main content

True polyandry and pseudopolyandry: why does a monandrous fly remate?

Abstract

Background

The rate of female remating can have important impacts on a species, from affecting conflict and cooperation within families, to population viability and gene flow. However, determining the level of polyandry in a species can be difficult, with information on the mating system of many species being based on a single experiment, or completely absent. Here we investigate the mating system of the fruit fly Drosophila subobscura. Reports from England, Spain and Canada suggest D. subobscura is entirely monandrous, with no females remating. However, work in Greece suggests that 23% of females remate. We examine the willingness of female D. subobscura to remate in the laboratory in a range of conditions, using flies from both Greece and England. We make a distinction between pseudopolyandry, where a female remates after an ineffective first mating that is incapable of fertilising her eggs, and true polyandry, where a female remates even though she has received suitable sperm from a previous mating.

Results

We find a low rate of true polyandry by females (4%), with no difference between populations. The rate of true polyandry is affected by temperature, but not starvation. Pseudopolyandry is three times as common as true polyandry, and most females showing pseudopolyandry mated at their first opportunity after their first failed mating. However, despite the lack of differences in polyandry between the populations, we do find differences in the way males respond to exposure to other males prior to mating. In line with previous work, English flies responded to one or more rivals by increasing their copulation duration, a response previously thought to be driven by sperm competition. Greek males only show increased copulation duration when exposed to four or more rival males. This suggests that the response to rivals in D. subobscura is not related to sperm competition, because sperm competition is rare, and there is no correlation of response to rivals and mating system across the populations.

Conclusions

These results illustrate the difficulties in determining the mating system of a species, even one that is well known and an excellent laboratory species, with results being highly dependent on the conditions used to assay the behaviour, and the population used.

Background

Female remating rate is extremely variable, ranging from females mating with a single male in their lifetime, to extreme polyandry where a female may mate with hundreds of males [1, 2]. These differences in mating system can have profound effects on a species, impacting on effective population size and population viability [3], the rate of gene flow [4], and the spread of sexually transmitted [5, 6] and genetic diseases [7]. Female remating rate also impacts on relatedness within families and hence the level of conflict, cooperation, and sociality within family groups [8, 9]. The mating system has a particularly strong impact on the degree of pre and post-copulatory choice by females [10], competition between males [11], male–female conflict [12], and subsequent adaptations to these pressures [13, 14].

However, despite the importance of polyandry and the decades of research into its causes and consequences, there is still considerable confusion over polyandry in the literature. The use of the word “polyandry” varies between papers, and difficulties in determining the level of polyandry in a species further confuse the issue. Some authors have used the term polyandry for instances where a female has remated after receiving sperm incapable of fertilising her e.g. [2, 1517] and thus where sperm competition cannot be taking place. However, we argue that almost all the key ecological and evolutionary impacts of polyandry (see previous paragraph) depend on the possibility of a female’s offspring being sired by multiple fathers [18]. In the many cases where offspring cannot be fathered by a male (due to the copulation being a short pseudocopulation without sperm transfer, male sterility, or genetic incompatibility preventing fertilisation) the consequences of the mating are greatly reduced. Only the spread of sexually transmitted diseases, the risk of mechanical damage to male or female, and prevention of infanticide by males will be affected equally by fertile and infertile copulations. In species where males provide resources to females during or after mating, such as nuptial gifts or parental care, females may gain from infertile matings with additional males. However, high costs to males are expected to drive choosiness and high fertility in males, potentially reducing the likelihood of infertile matings in such species. Despite these potential costs and benefits of infertile matings, we argue that the presence of sperm competition is the factor that makes the most important difference between polyandry and monandry (generally defined as “females mating with a single male”), and that it would be useful to clarify the terminology around polyandry to reflect this.

We suggest the convention that “true polyandry” refers to females mating with more than one male after which sperm competition occurs. In contrast, “pseudopolyandry” refers to females mating with more than one male where sperm competition does not occur. This convention would leave “polyandry” as a general term covering all situations where a female mates with multiple males. Throughout this paper, this is the convention we will use. Obviously in many cases it will be difficult to distinguish true polyandry from pseudopolyandry. Nevertheless, we think it is important for biologists to have clearly defined concepts to use if their data allow it.

Even when using the broadest sense of monandry and polyandry, determining the mating system of a species is often difficult. Typically the mating system of a species is determined using a relatively small number of observations, either in the field or laboratory, or with paternity analysis using genetic techniques [19, 20], often from a single location and time point [21]. However, the number of times a female mates may vary between populations [22], environmental conditions [23], and both the physical condition [24, 25] and genotype [2628] of the female. As a very simple example, if a particular population has an unusually short female lifespan, it may be impossible for a normally polyandrous species to mate more than once, forcing it to become monandrous under those circumstances. As a result, many species referred to as “polyandrous” or “monandrous” may show considerable variation, and in many cases the assigned mating system may simply be wrong.

Here we investigate the mating system of a well-studied species, the fruit fly D. subobscura, a model species for research into evolution [29], genetics [30], ecology [31] and climate change [32]. However, despite decades of research there is still considerable confusion about the mating system in this species. It has generally been thought to be completely monandrous, with evidence coming from studies conducted on populations from England [33], Spain [34] and Canada [35]. However, a study on a population from Greece detected insemination by at least two males in about 20% of wild-caught and mass laboratory reared females, with the authors estimating that all wild females may mate multiple times [36]. Further evidence suggesting this species is polyandrous comes from a study on male investment in mating after exposure to a potential rival male. Lizé and colleagues [35] found that male D. subobscura that were reared with one or more males had longer copulation durations than males reared in isolation. This increase in copulation duration when males are exposed to potential rivals is seen in many Drosophila species [35, 37] and has been interpreted as a response to the increased risk of sperm competition. Work on D. melanogaster[37] and D. pseudoobscura[38] has shown that this increase in copulation effort increases offspring production and success in sperm competition. In D. pseudoobscura, males exposed to rivals increase the number of fertilising sperm transferred to the female [38]. In D. melanogaster males exposed to rivals transfer more sperm to females, but only if their hearing and taste senses function normally [39]. In D. melanogaster the transcription and transfer of seminal fluid proteins also increases after exposure to rivals [40], although there is some evidence that actual production of seminal proteins may drop [41]. If the response of D. subobscura males to rivals is a response to sperm competition, it suggests that polyandry must be present in many populations.

We tested for remating in D. subobscura in two populations, one from the UK (thought to be monandrous) and one from Greece (thought to be polyandrous), under a range of laboratory conditions. As D. subobscura males often feed females prior to mating [42, 43], we included low nutrition conditions. We also manipulated male exposure to potential rival males, and observed their subsequent mating behaviour to test whether there is variation in the response to rivals in this species, and whether this correlates with polyandry as predicted by theory [37]. In line with our definitions above, we classified females as showing true polyandry when they remated after a fertile mating (one that produced offspring). If a female remated after producing no offspring from her first mating, we classified this as pseudopolyandry. See methods for further details.

Results

See Table 1 for the percentages of females showing true polyandry, pseudopolyandry, and overall remating rate in each of the different conditions.

Table 1 Female remating behaviour of the two populations in different conditions

Population differences in mating rate

The population of the female did not affect the rate of true polyandry (GLM, χ21, 372 =0.004, p = 0.948), and neither did the population of the male in the first mating (GLM, χ21, 372 = 0.034, p = 0.855) nor the interaction between male and female populations (GLM, χ21, 372 = 0.464, p = 0.496). The same was true when examining pseudopolyandry, (GLM, population of the female: χ21, 374 = 0.020, p = 0.887; male: χ21, 373 = 0.023, p = 0.879; interaction: χ21, 372 = 0.002, p = 0.966). No factors significantly predicted the number of days until remating, either for true polyandry (p > 0.648 in all cases) or pseudopolyandry (p > 0.278 in all cases).

Impact of temperature on remating

The rate of true polyandry was affected by a significant interaction between the temperature the female was kept at and the population of the cross (GLM, χ21, 394 = 4.246, p = 0.039). The rate of true polyandry in UK flies showed a linear increase with increasing temperature, whereas for the Greek flies there was a non-significant U-shaped trend, with the lowest levels of polyandry at 21°C. UK flies showed significant differences in rate of true polyandry at different female storage temperatures (0%, 4.4% and 8.3% at 18°C, 21°C and 25°C respectively; Pearson’s Chi-squared test, χ22 = 6.247, p = 0.044) driven by a difference between the 18 and 25°C treatments (Pearson’s Chi-squared test with Yates’ continuity correction, 18–21 p = 0.171, 18–25 p = 0.034, 21–25 p = 0.437). This difference in true polyandry was not detected in the Greek flies (7.5%, 3.8% and 5.7% at 18°C, 21°C and 25°C respectively; Pearson’s Chi-squared test, χ22 = 1.064, p = 0.588). There was a trend for the rate of true polyandry to increase at higher mating temperatures, but this was marginally non-significant (GLM, χ21, 394 = 3.429, p = 0.064). The temperature the male was kept at did not significantly predict rate of true polyandry (GLM, χ21, 394 = 0.400, p = 0.527). All other factors were non-significant (p >0.121 in all cases).

The rate of pseudopolyandry was affected by an interaction between the temperature the male was kept at and the population of the cross, although this was very marginally non-significant (GLM, χ21, 479 = 3.816, p = 0.051). Greek females tended to be more likely to be pseudopolyandrous if the male was stored at a higher temperature (5.7%, 9.3% and 13.9% at 18°C, 21°C and 25°C respectively), while UK females showed the reverse trend of being less pseudopolyandrous if the male was kept at a higher temperature (12.2%, 9.7% and 7.3% at 18°C, 21°C and 25°C respectively). If the interaction is considered non-significant, then none of the other factors affected the rate of pseudopolyandry (GLM, female storage temperature: χ21, 478 = 0.778, p = 0.378; male storage temperature: χ21, 481 = 0.137, p = 0.712; temperature the mating occurred at: χ21, 476 = 0.941, p = 0.332). The population of the cross was not significant (GLM, χ21, 480 = 0.015, p = 0.903). No other factors were significant (p > 0.248 in all cases).

None of the temperature factors significantly predicted refractory period for polyandry (GLM, p > 0.259 in all cases).The refractory period for pseudopolyandry was affected by a significant interaction between the population of the cross, the temperature the female was kept at and the temperature the mating occurred at (GLM, χ23, 45 = 4.541, p = 0.033). All other factors were not significant (p > 0.436 in all cases). The total number of offspring produced tended to increase when the mating took place at a higher temperature, although this was non-significant (GLM, χ21, 476 = 3.704, p = 0.054). If that factor is removed males kept at high temperatures sired more offspring (GLM, χ21, 477 = 21.052, p < 0.001) while females kept at higher temperatures produced fewer offspring (GLM, χ21, 477 = 12.783, p < 0.001). None of the interactions were significant for total offspring production (p > 0.250 in all cases).

Hunger and remating

Few Greek females mated initially when starved, so they were removed from the analyses for remating behaviour (N = 17). Hunger state of the male and female were not significant factors for predicting rate of true polyandry (GLM: female: χ21, 351 = 0.058, p = 0.810; male: χ21, 350 < 0.001, p = 0.998), or the rate of pseudopolyandry (GLM, female: χ21, 348 = 0.221, p = 0.639; male: χ21, 351 = 0.653, p = 0.419. Population also did not affect polyandry (true polyandry: GLM, χ21, 348 = 0.156, p = 0.693; pseudopolyandry: GLM, χ21, 350 = 0.090, p = 0.765). All other factors were non-significant (polyandry: p > 0.200; pseudopolyandry: p > 0.553in all cases). Analysis of refractory period for polyandry was not possible due to lack of variation in refractory period for polyandrous females. No factors affected the refractory period for pseudopolyandry (p > 0.096 in all cases). Females on a low nutrient food produced fewer offspring (populations pooled, means of 73 and 33 offspring for fed and starved females respectively; GLM, χ21, 235 = 81.141, p < 0.001). All other factors were non-significant for total offspring produced (p > 0.096 in all cases).

Polyandry over time

Decreasing numbers of flies were pseudopolyandrous at each opportunity (Figure 1; all data pooled, 71/893, 19/805 and 2/795 remating at three, seven and ten days after the original mating respectively; Chi-squared test, χ22 = 85.091, p < 0.001). All pairs were statistically significant (pair-wise Chi-squared tests, p < 0.001 in all cases). The rate of true polyandry did not change over time, (all data pooled, 17/893, 12/805 and 8/795 females showing true polyandry at three, seven and ten days after the mating respectively; Chi-squared test, χ22 = 3.707, p = 0.157). The refractory period for pseudopolyandry was significantly shorter than the refractory period before polyandry (Figure 1, all data pooled; N = 129, medians of 3 and 7 days respectively, Mann–Whitney U-test, W = 1106.5, p < 0.001).

Figure 1
figure 1

Number of females remating at each opportunity. Data from all remating experiments pooled, in total 794 mated females. Females that remated but never produced offspring are not included as their mating status could not be determined (N = 99).

Latency to mating and offspring production over all experiments

Latency before the original mating did not differ between females that went on to remate and those that never remated (all data pooled, log10 transformed, N = 893, Welch’s t-test, t1, 208 = −0.088, p = 0.930). Copulation duration of the original mating did not differ between females that went on to remate and those that never remated (all data pooled, square root transformed, N = 893, Welch’s t-test, t 1, 188 = 0.617, p = 0.538). A significant difference was found in the total number of offspring produced between females of different mating status (means of 86, 64 and 70 offspring for monandrous, pseudopolyandrous and true polyandrous females respectively, N = 893, Kruskal-Wallis test, χ22 =12.947, p = 0.002). Monandrous females produced significantly more offspring than those that were pseudopolyandrous (N = 757, Mann–Whitney U-test, W = 37279.5, p < 0.001). Polyandrous females produced a similar number of offspring to pseudopolyandrous females (N = 129, Mann–Whitney U-test, W = 1542.5, p = 0.408) and monandrous females (N = 702, Mann–Whitney U-test, W = 14013.0, p = 0.154). There was no difference in offspring production between the populations (all data pooled, medians of 79 & 69 for UK and Greek females respectively; Mann–Whitney U-test, W = 72429, p = 0.080).

Male response to rivals

In both UK and Greek populations, copulation duration varied significantly between different levels of male competition (Figure 2; GLM: UK, F3, 163 = 12.113, p <0.001; Greece, F3, 192 = 11.093, p < 0.001). However, the threshold of response varied between the two populations. The copulation duration of UK males exposed to any number of rivals (from one to nine rivals) was significantly higher than that of males kept alone prior to mating (Tukey post-hoc comparisons with lone male: vs. one rival, p < 0.005; vs. four rivals, p = 0.030; vs. nine rivals, p < 0.005). The copulation duration of Greek males was only increased when males were exposed to four or more rivals prior to mating (Tukey post-hoc comparisons with lone male: vs. one rival, p = 0.741; vs. four rivals, p < 0.005; vs. nine rivals, p < 0.005). These differences remain significant if the Bonferroni correction for multiple tests is used. The number of rivals a male was exposed to in pre-copulation treatments did not affect the subsequent offspring production of the female. This was true of both UK (Kruskal-Wallis: H = 3.27, p = 0.663, N = 69) and Greek (GLM: F3, 53 = 2.424, p = 0.076) flies.

Figure 2
figure 2

Boxplot showing the copulation duration of UK and Greek flies with different numbers of rivals. Flies were either kept alone (white), or exposed to one rival (light-grey), four rivals (medium-grey) or nine rivals (dark-grey) prior to mating. Plots indicate the median, interquartile range, and range. Notches indicate 95% confidence intervals [44].

Discussion

As predicted by other studies [3335], very few D. subobscura from either population remated. We found no evidence of a high willingness to remate in the Greek population, despite the report of frequent polyandry in the laboratory and field [36]. The majority of all rematings occurred when the female failed to produce offspring from her first mating. True polyandry was considerably rarer, with an overall rate of 4% over all experiments and no overall difference between the populations, although UK females showed an increase in likelihood of true polyandry when they were kept at higher temperatures. However, we did find a difference in the response of males to exposure to potential male rivals. The typical Drosophila response to rivals of increased copulation duration [35, 37] was only shown by Greek males when exposed to four or more rivals, whereas the UK males responded to a single rival. This difference in response is the first evidence of intraspecific variation in this behaviour. However, as D. subobscura appears to be largely monandrous, this difference in response to rivals is unlikely to be related to the risk of sperm competition.

One potential inaccuracy in our methods is that we may have missed some females that showed true polyandry. If a female had a fertile first mating, but did not produce offspring before remating, she would be scored as showing pseudopolyandry. This is unlikely to be a major problem, because the females we used were seven days old, and should have been fully reproductively mature. Estimates of the survival of D. subobscura[45] and relatives [46, 47] in the wild suggest that most flies die within ten days and few live more than three weeks. As a result, failing to oviposit for three days is likely to be costly to a female, and unlikely to be adaptive. We observed that overall 12% of monandrous females failed to produce offspring in the first three days but then went on to produce offspring. Hence at most 1.2% of females would have been wrongly assigned as pseudopolyandrous when they really showed true polyandry, increasing overall levels of true polyandry from 4 to 5.2%.

Although our results broadly concur with previous work that suggests D. subobscura is monandrous, discrepancies remain. Previous work by several research groups found no evidence of remating in D. subobscura at all [3335] although Maynard Smith [33] did find that mated but uninseminated females were willing to remate (pseudopolyandry). The most likely explanation for this is that these studies all used small sample sizes (N <51 in all cases), so may have missed relatively rare remating events. In addition temperature may have played a role. Maynard Smith conducted his experiments at 18-20°C, and found no true polyandry [33], and our UK flies showed no true polyandry at 18°C. This demonstrates the importance of repeating studies in different conditions, as otherwise we would not have detected polyandry in the UK population. The high level of true polyandry reported by Loukas et al.[36] in both wild caught and laboratory flies is more difficult to explain. In our results, starvation does not affect the rate of true polyandry, but temperature does. Unfortunately Loukas et al. do not report the temperature at which their flies were maintained. The flies used in the experiment were the F2 progeny of wild flies trapped in August 1980 [36], so the experiment must have been carried out sometime between October and February. Hence a wide range of temperatures would be possible. Given that we observed higher rates of polyandry at both 18°C and 25°C, temperature may explain their laboratory results.

The high rate of true polyandry they detected in the wild caught flies is more difficult to explain. One possibility is that many of the wild caught females were old, and so had had more opportunities to remate. However, our experiment covered two and a half weeks, which should include the majority of most females’ lives [45]. Alternatively, females in nature may remate because they are exposed to a wider selection of males, and remate only when courted by very high quality males (“trading up”) [48]. However, our presentation of three males to each mated female makes it likely that at least one male would be superior to her original mate. In a similar vein, females in nature are likely to be harassed by males almost continuously when ovipositing (Price, pers. obs.). Females might remate in nature to reduce harassment, although this seems unlikely as females are often harassed by mutliple males, so mating with one will not help, and recently mated females are still harassed. It is also possible that the flies Loukas et al. used were different to ours. We collected from the same location which has been preserved as a nature reserve and has had a consistent ecology. Nevertheless, the 30 year gap between collections could have allowed an evolutionary change in mating system. Unfortunately, testing this would require access to the flies Loukas et al. used, and an in depth knowledge of the genetic basis of mating system in this species, neither of which is available at present. Finally, it is possible that a complex suite of factors such as temperature, length of reproductive season and condition of both males and females act together to increase rate of true polyandry in the Greek population, that cannot be easily replicated in the laboratory [22, 25, 49].

If we accept that our estimates of true polyandry are solid, then there is still the question of whether D. subobscura can be considered monandrous. Firstly, can a species showing up to 8% polyandry in controlled laboratory experiments be called monandrous? A strict definition of monandry, where no individuals ever remate, would be extremely difficult to prove for any species, and so would restrict the use of the category to theory. Furthermore, all animals make mistakes, and so even a species that has been strongly selected for monandry would be likely to remate in at least some unusual circumstances. In practice, there is an enormous amount of variation in which species are described as monandrous. Some authors describe a species where up to 25% of the females act polyandrously as a monandrous species [50]. Over all experiments, D. subobscura showed a four percent rate of true polyandry, but this varied between conditions, most strongly with temperature. This illustrates the difficulties of determining the mating system of a species from laboratory experiments. Overall, the four percent remating rate of D. subobscura probably does make it reasonable to call it monandrous [51]. However our laboratory estimates are still contradicted by Loukas et al., who estimated 23% multiple paternity in nature. New measurements of polyandry in natural populations, using modern molecular techniques would be useful to resolve this.

The majority of rematings (71%) that we observed were pseudopolyandry. The rate of pseudopolyandry followed a very different pattern to that of true polyandry, showing a strong decrease in frequency with time. In serially true polyandrous species the propensity to remate is expected to increase over time [52, 53], although in our experiments true polyandry remained constant over time. If females can detect that their first mating will not produce offspring, perhaps due to a failure of sperm transfer, male infertility, or genetic incompatibility, then remating as early as possible will increase her fitness. As predicted, in this study females that were pseudopolyandrous were far more likely to remate at their first opportunity than the second or third.

The contrasting impacts of both time and temperature on pseudopolyandry compared to true polyandry suggests that the two are indeed distinct behaviours, likely to be driven by different evolutionary pressures and controlled by different mechanisms. Moreover, the consequences of pseudopolyandry are likely to be far less wide ranging than those of true polyandry. The area where pseudopolyandry is likely to be most important is in disease transmission and epidemiology, where fertile matings are not required [54]. Pseudopolyandry is likely to be very widespread, with work from a wide variety of species showing that females are likely to remate if their first mating is infertile [49, 5560]. However, determining whether a species shows true or pseudopolyandry is likely to be difficult for many species, requiring extensive behavioural information on the species, best supported with molecular investigation of multiple paternity [61]. Genetic incompatibility provides a further complication, with some crosses between individuals likely to show greatly reduced offspring fitness, but not necessarily complete failure [17]. In addition, rates and drivers of both true and pseudopolyandry are likely to differ between populations, and between environments, as shown by some of the experiments presented here. Nevertheless, we believe that despite the difficulties of accurately determining the mating system, and potential complications due to interactions between individuals, the terms “true polyandry” and “pseudopolyandry” will add useful clarity to the field.

If D. subobscura is indeed a largely monandrous species, then why do males show an increase in copulation duration when exposed to potential mating rivals? The threat of sperm competition is likely to be very low for these males, even if the 23% true polyandry rate is true. One possibility is that males are responding to the likelihood of mating with more than one female. If females are monandrous, the chance of encountering a female that is willing to mate is lower when rivals are present. Hence males may maximise their fitness by investing all their reserves in the first female they mate with [62]. Alternatively, the response may not be adaptive, but might be due to conflict between males [63]. Longer copulation durations in D. subobscura may be due to exhaustion or damage in males exposed to rivals. However, we currently do not know why males from the two populations should show different patterns of response to rivals. All previous studies have found that all males respond to the presence of one or more rivals with the same increase in copulation duration [35, 37, 64, 65]. The only exception is D. bifasciata, in which males show no response to rivals at all, irrespective of how many are present [66]. The difference between the populations in their response to rivals, with no corresponding difference in female willingness to remate, suggests that this response is not related to sperm competition in this species. Copulation duration and amount of sperm transferred are not necessarily related in D. melanogaster either [39, 67]. What does drive the difference in male response in D. subobscura is currently unknown. There are a great many possible ecological differences between the UK and Greek populations. For example, population density may differ between populations, causing different rates of male-male contact and aggression [68]. Our present paucity of information about the two populations makes it difficult to identify the cause, now that it is unlikely to be sperm competition.

Conclusions

Overall, our results suggest that D. subobscura is a generally monandrous species, although further estimates of polyandry in natural populations would be useful to confirm this. Previous differences in estimates of polyandry were probably driven by differences in the methods used to assay remating, compounded by small sample sizes. However, our two populations did differ in the response males shown to exposure to potential rivals, making it unlikely that this response is related to the risk of sperm competition, as predicted by previous studies. As found in other species, some female D. subobscura remate after a mating that failed to fertilise them (pseudopolyandry), while only a small proportion exhibit true polyandry. We suggest that this distinction between true polyandry and pseudopolyandry is one that is worth making in future studies of female remating behaviour, especially when conditions may affect the two behaviours differently.

Methods

Experimental populations

We collected females of D. subobscura from Adel Dam, Leeds, UK (53°52′1.30″N, 1°35′7.83″E) in May 2011 (“UK” flies) and from Parnitha National Park, Athens, Greece (38°10′24.24″N, 23°43′2.64″E) in April 2011 (“Greek” flies). Flies were maintained as mixed populations in the laboratory (N = 200 per generation), descended from 24 and 27 wild females for the UK and Greek populations respectively. We maintained them on a maize, sugar, agar and yeast medium at 21°C [42] with a 14:10 hr photoperiod (lights on at 10:00 GMT). We conducted all experiments in these conditions unless stated otherwise.

Population differences in remating

To examine the willingness of females from both populations to remate, we first mated virgin females to males, and then presented them with regular opportunities to mate with another male. For each population we collected flies as one day old virgins under CO2 anaesthesia and transferred them to single sex groups of 15–20 flies per vial. We mated all flies at eight days old, by which time D. subobscura are sexually mature [34] and unaffected by the prior CO2 anaesthesia [69]. At seven days old we aspirated each female into an individual vial. This increases mating propensity in females as they acclimatise to the vial [69]. Males were also placed in a vial on their own one day before they would be used in a mating, which improves their chance of mating [33], although the mechanism for this is unclear. We began each mating and remating experiment within 15 minutes of the light period of our photoperiod beginning. This provides a “dawn” stimulus, when D. subobscura are most active in nature [70]. We aspirated a male into a vial containing a single female, and observed the pair for two hours, recording the time to start of mating (latency) and copulation duration. A period of two hours was used for several reasons. Firstly, 88% of the D. subobscura that mate within two hours mate within the first 60 minutes. Secondly, D. subobscura are thought to be most active for a few hours just after dawn and before dusk, making two hours a reasonable estimate of an interaction period. Once a mating ended we discarded the male. For the first matings, we discarded any females that were unmated after two hours. A mean of 56% females failed to mate at the first opportunity, and this ranged from 32% to 86% for different experiments. This low mating rate by virgin D. subobscura is not unusual for the species, having been reported in previous experiments [43]. However, it does mean that the females that did mate are unlikely to be an entirely random sample of females, particularly in the female starved treatments where very few females mated. It is possible that females that did not remate on day 8 would have mated later if given the opportunity, and might show different remating behaviour to the initially mated females. Our experimental design cannot directly address this, although we are not aware of any previous work that has suggested such changes are likely in Drosophila. Once mated, we then gave females remating opportunities with new seven day old virgin males on three occasions, at three, seven and ten days after the original mating. The remating opportunity lasted two hours, after which we discarded the males.

We moved all females into new vials with fresh food three days after the original mating opportunity, immediately after the second mating opportunity, and ten days after the original mating, immediately after the final remating opportunity. Drosophila of the Obscura group show an insemination reaction on mating in which the ejaculate becomes a gelatinous mass for approximately 30 minutes after mating, and sperm cannot be used by the female to fertilise eggs [71]. Hence any offspring in the first vial cannot have been fathered by the second male, and would indicate that the first mating was fertile. We discarded the females after 17 days, and we monitored the vials they were kept in for emerging adults. One week after the majority of vials in a condition had the first adult emerge, we counted the total number of adult offspring in each vial. Females that remated after producing offspring were recorded as showing true polyandry. If a remating occurred before the female produced any offspring we termed it pseudopolyandry. All females were mated only to males from the same population.

Examination of which sex controls remating

To determine whether the possible differences between populations in remating rate were male or female controlled, we repeated the experiment above, but initially mated half the females with a male from the same population, and half with a male from the other population Remating opportunities were with males of alternating populations. This was the only experiment that crossed males and females of different populations. All subsequent experiments crossed males and females from the same population.

Effect of temperature on remating

To detect any effect of temperature on remating behaviour, we repeated the original design independently at 18°C and 25°C. We collected virgins from vials maintained at 21°C as above, but kept the adults at either 18°C or 25°C. We carried out the matings at the temperature the males and females were raised at i.e. 18°C or 25°C. Crosses with adult flies kept at 18°C or 25°C were also performed at 21°C, to control for the temperature during courtship/mating, which can affect the degree of remating [72]. To establish whether any effect of temperature was male or female controlled, we kept females at 18°C and mated them with males from 25°C, and we mated females kept at 25°C with males from 18°C. The matings for these crosses occurred at 21°C. All remating opportunities were with males raised at the same temperature as the male in the original mating and occurred at the same temperature as the original mating.

Effect of manipulating hunger state

To determine whether flies in a worse condition had altered mating and remating behaviour, we altered the diet of males and females. In order to starve the females we kept them on food containing 1/5 of the yeast of the standard food from collection as one day old virgins for their entire adult lives. This degree of starvation altered remating behaviour in D. melanogaster[25]. To starve the males we aspirated them at six days old into an individual vial containing a piece of blue paper over damp cotton wool, providing water but no nutrition [43]. We mated the flies at seven days old. We crossed females, either starved or in normal condition, with males, in starved or normal condition. All remating opportunities were with males in normal condition (not starved).

Male response to rivals

To examine the response of males to exposure to potential mating rivals, we placed one day old virgin males either alone or with one, four or nine rival males. Males were kept in these conditions until they were transferred to the female vial for mating. A single male was used from each vial to avoid pseudoreplication [35], and males from vials containing dead males were not used. Virgin females were collected as above. As standard, we recorded the occurrence of mating, latency and duration of copulation. Each pair was allowed two hours to mate, after which we discarded males. As in the first experiment, mated females were then retained for offspring counts. We repeated this process independently for both Greek and UK populations.

Data analysis

Data were analysed using IBM SPSS v20 and R 2.15.1 [73]. Females that remated but produced no offspring at all were excluded from analyses, as the female might have been infertile; in which case determining whether she showed true polyandry or pseudopolyandry was not possible. Where Generalised Linear Models were used, we constructed the maximal model and then removed non-significant factors in a stepwise manner.

References

  1. Arnqvist G, Nilsson T: The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim Behav. 2000, 60 (2): 145-164. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Simmons LW: The evolution of polyandry: an examination of the genetic incompatability and good-sperm hypotheses. J Evol Biol. 2001, 14: 585-594. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00309.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Holman L, Kokko H: The consequences of polyandry for population viability, extinction risk and conservation. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2013, 368: 20120053-10.1098/rstb.2012.0053.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Jaffé R, Moritz RFA, Kraus FB: Gene flow is maintained by polyandry and male dispersal in the army ant Eciton burchellii. Popul Ecol. 2009, 51: 227-236. 10.1007/s10144-008-0133-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Sherman PW, Seeley TD, Reeve HK: Parasites, pathogens and polyandry in social hymenoptera. Am Nat. 1988, 131 (4): 602-610. 10.1086/284809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Poiani A, Wilks C: Sexually transmitted diseases: a possible cost of promiscuity in birds. Auk. 2000, 117 (4): 1061-1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Price TA, Hurst GD, Wedell N: Polyandry prevents extinction. Current biology : CB. 2010, 20 (5): 471-475. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.050.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Trivers RL: Parent-offspring conflict. Amer Zool. 1974, 14: 249-264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hughes WO, Oldroyd BP, Beekman M, Ratnieks FL: Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality. Science. 2008, 320 (5880): 1213-1216. 10.1126/science.1156108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Thornhill R: Cryptic female choice and its implications in the scorpionfly Harpobittacs nigriceps. Am Nat. 1983, 122 (6): 765-788. 10.1086/284170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Parker GA: Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol Rev. 1970, 45: 525-567. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1970.tb01176.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hosken DJ, Stockley P, Tregenza T, Wedell N: Monogamy and the battle of the sexes. Annu Rev Entomol. 2009, 54: 361-378. 10.1146/annurev.ento.54.110807.090608.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rowe L, Arnqvist G, Sih A, Krupa JJ: Sexual conflict and the evolutionary ecology of mating patterns: water striders as a model system. Trends Ecol Evol. 1994, 9: 289-293. 10.1016/0169-5347(94)90032-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Parker GA: Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006, 361 (1466): 235-259. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1785.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mossinson S, Yuval B: Regulation of sexual receptivity of female Mediterranean fruit flies: old hypotheses revisited and a new synthesis proposed. J Insect Physiol. 2003, 49 (6): 561-567. 10.1016/S0022-1910(03)00027-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zeh JA, Zeh DW: The evolution of polyandry I: intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatability. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1996, 263 (1377): 1711-1717. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Zeh JA, Zeh DW: The evolution of polyandry II: post-copulatory defences against genetic incompatability. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1997, 264: 69-75. 10.1098/rspb.1997.0010.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kvarnemo C, Simmons LW: Polyandry as a mediator of sexual selection before and after mating. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013, 368 (1613): 20120042-10.1098/rstb.2012.0042.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Westneat DF: Extra-pair copulations in a predominately monogamous birds: observations of behaviour. Anim Behav. 1987, 35: 865-876. 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80122-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Haddrill PR, Shuker DM, Amos W, Majerus ME, Mayes S: Female multiple mating in wild and laboratory populations of the two-spot ladybird, Adalia bipunctata. Mol Ecol. 2008, 17 (13): 3189-3197. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03812.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Simmons LW: Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. 2001, Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  22. Välimäki P, Kaitala A, Kokko H: Temporal patterns in reproduction may explain variationin mating frequencies in the green-veined white butterfly Pieris napi. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006, 61 (1): 99-107. 10.1007/s00265-006-0240-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. McFarlane SE, Lane JE, Taylor RW, Gorrell JC, Coltman DW, Humphries MM, Boutin S, McAdam AG: The heritability of multiple male mating in a promiscuous mammal. Biol Lett. 2011, 7 (3): 368-371. 10.1098/rsbl.2010.1003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Syriatowicz A, Brooks R: Sexual responsiveness is condition-dependent in female guppies, but preference functions are not. BMC Ecol. 2004, 4: 5-10.1186/1472-6785-4-5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Fricke C, Bretman A, Chapman T: Female nutritional status determines the magnitude and sign of responses to a male ejaculate signal in Drosophila melanogaster. J Evol Biol. 2010, 23 (1): 157-165. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01882.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Välimäki P, Kaitala A: Does a lack of mating opportunites explain monandry in the green-veined white butterly (Pieris napi)?. Oikos. 2006, 115: 110-116. 10.1111/j.2006.0030-1299.14947.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Harano T, Miyatake T: Heritable variation in polyandry in Callosobruchus chinensis. Anim Behav. 2005, 70 (2): 299-304. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.10.023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Price TAR, Lewis Z, Smith DT, Hurst GDD, Wedell N: Remating in the laboratory reflects rates of polyandry in the wild. Anim Behav. 2011, 82 (6): 1381-1386. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.09.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Latorre A, Moya A, Ayala FJ: Evolution of mitrochondrial DNA in Drosophila subobscura. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1986, 83: 8649-8653. 10.1073/pnas.83.22.8649.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Huey RB, Gilchrist GW, Carlson ML, Berrigan D, Serra L: Rapid evolution of a geographic cline in size in an introduced fly. Science. 2000, 287: 308-309. 10.1126/science.287.5451.308.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Davis JR, Jenkinson LS, Lawton JH, Shorrocks B, Wood S: Making mistakes when predicting shifts in species range in response to global warming. Nature. 1998, 391: 783-786. 10.1038/35842.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Balanya J, Oller JM, Huey RB, Gilchrist GW, Serra L: Global genetic change tracks global climate warming in Drosophila subobscura. Science. 2006, 313 (5794): 1773-1775. 10.1126/science.1131002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Maynard Smith J: Fertility, mating behaviour and sexual selection in Drosophila subobscura. J Genet. 1956, 54 (2): 261-279. 10.1007/BF02982781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Holman L, Freckleton RP, Snook RR: What use is an infertile sperm? A comparative study of sperm-heteromorphic drosophila. Evolution. 2008, 62 (2): 374-385. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00280.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lizé A, Doff RJ, Smaller EA, Lewis Z, Hurst GD: Perception of male-male competition influences Drosophila copulation behaviour even in species where females rarely remate. Biol Lett. 2012, 8 (1): 35-38. 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0544.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Loukas M, Vergini Y, Krimbas CB: The genetics of Drosophila subobscura populatios XVIII. Multiple insemination and sperm displacement in Drosophila subobscura. Genetica. 1981, 57: 29-37. 10.1007/BF00057540.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Bretman A, Fricke C, Chapman T: Plastic responses of male Drosophila melanogaster to the level of sperm competition increase male reproductive fitness. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2009, 276 (1662): 1705-1711. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1878.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Price TAR, Lizé A, Marcello M, Bretman A: Experience of mating rivals causes males to modulate sperm transfer in the fly Drosophila pseudoobscura. J Insect Physiol. 2012, 58: 1669-1675. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.10.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Garbaczewska M, Billeter JC, Levine JD: Drosophila melanogaster males increase the number of sperm in their ejaculate when perceiving rival males. J Insect Physiol. 2013, 59 (3): 306-310. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.08.016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Wigby S, Sirot LK, Linklater JR, Buehner N, Calboli FC, Bretman A, Wolfner MF, Chapman T: Seminal fluid protein allocation and male reproductive success. Current biology: CB. 2009, 19 (9): 751-757. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Fedorka KM, Winterhalter WE, Ware B: Perceived sperm competition intensity influences seminal fluid protein production prior to courtship and mating. Evolution. 2011, 65 (2): 584-590. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01141.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Steele RH: Courtship feeding in Drosophila subobscura. I. The nutritional significance of courtship feeding. Anim Behav. 1986, 34: 1087-1098. 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80168-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Immonen E, Hoikkala A, Kazem AJN, Ritchie MG: When are vomiting males attractive? Sexual selection on condition-dependent nuptial feeding in Drosophila subobscura. Behav Ecol. 2009, 20 (2): 289-295. 10.1093/beheco/arp008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. McGill R, Tukey JW, Larsen WA: Variations of box plots. Am Stat. 1978, 32 (1): 12-16.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Rosewell J, Shorrocks B: The implication of survival rates in natural populations of Drosophila: capture-recapture experiments on domestic speceis. Biol J Linn Soc. 1987, 32: 373-384. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1987.tb00438.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Powell J: Progress and prospects in evolutionary biology: the Drosophila model. 1997, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  47. Dobzhansky T, Wright S: Genetics of natural populations X. Dispersion rates in Drosophila pseudoobscura. Genetics. 1943, 28: 304-340.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Halliday TR: The study of mate choice. Mate choice. Edited by: Bateson P. 1983, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  49. Gavriel S, Gazit Y, Yuval B: Remating by female Mediterranean fruit flies (Ceratitis capitata, Diptera: Tephritidae): temporal patterns and modulation by male condition. J Insect Physiol. 2009, 55 (7): 637-642. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2009.04.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Torres-Vila LM, Rodriguez-Molina MC, Jennions MD: Polyandry and fecundity in the Lepidoptera: can methodological and conceptual approaches bias outcomes?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004, 55 (4): 315-324. 10.1007/s00265-003-0712-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Arnqvist G: Comparative evidence for the evolution of genitalia by sexual selection. Nature. 1998, 393: 784-786. 10.1038/31689.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Thornhill R, Alcock J: The evolution of insect mating systems. 1983, Cambridge: Harvard University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  53. Eberhard WG: Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. 1996, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  54. Ashby B, Gupta S: Sexually transmitted infections in polygamous mating systems. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2013, 368: 20120048-10.1098/rstb.2012.0048.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Xue L, Noll M: Drosophila female sexual behavior induced by sterile males showing copulation complementation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000, 97 (7): 3272-3275. 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3272.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Katiyar KP, Ramirez E: Mating frequency and fertility of mediterranean fruit fly females alternately mated with normal and irradiated males. J Econ Entomol. 1970, 63 (4): 1248-1250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Chapman T, Miyatake T, Smith HK, Partridge L: Interactions of mating, egg production and death rates in females of the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 1998, 265 (1408): 1879-1894. 10.1098/rspb.1998.0516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Shelley TE, Kennelly S: Influence of male diet on male mating success and longevity and female remating in the Medeterranean fruit fly (Diptera: Tephritidae) under laboratory conditions. Florida Entomol. 2002, 85 (4): 572-579. 10.1653/0015-4040(2002)085[0572:IOMDOM]2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Proshold FI: Remating by Gypsy Moths (Lepidoptera: Lymantriidae) Mated wth F1-Sterile Males as a Function of Sperm Within the Spermatheca. J Econ Entomol. 1995, 88 (3): 644-648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Taylor OR: Relationship of Multiple mating to Fertility in Atteva punctella (Lepidoptera: Yponomeutidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1967, 60 (3): 583-590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Bretman A, Tregenza T: Measuring polyandry in wild populations: a case study using promiscuous crickets. Mol Ecol. 2005, 14 (7): 2169-2179. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02556.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Kokko H, Jennions MD: Parental investment, sexual selection and sex ratios. J Evol Biol. 2008, 21 (4): 919-948. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01540.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Chen S, Lee AY, Bowens NM, Huber R, Kravitz EA: Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002, 99 (8): 5664-5668. 10.1073/pnas.082102599.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Bretman A, Fricke C, Hetherington P, Stone R, Chapman T: Exposure to rivals and plastic responses to sperm competition in Drosophila melanogaster. Behav Ecol. 2010, 21 (2): 317-321. 10.1093/beheco/arp189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Bretman A, Gage MJ, Chapman T: Quick-change artists: male plastic behavioural responses to rivals. Trends Ecol Evol. 2011, 26 (9): 467-473. 10.1016/j.tree.2011.05.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Lizé A, Price TAR, Marcello M, Smaller EA, Lewis Z, Hurst GDD: Males do not prolong copulation in response to competitor males in the polyandrous fly Drosophila bifasciata. Physiol Entomol. 2012, 37 (3): 227-232. 10.1111/j.1365-3032.2012.00836.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Gilchrist AS, Partridge L: Why it is difficult to model sperm displacement in Drosophila melanogaster: the realtionship between sperm transfer and copulation duration. Evolution. 2000, 54 (2): 534-542.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Cushing BS, Razzoli M, Murphy AZ, Epperson PM, Le WW, Hoffman GE: Intraspecific variation in estrogen receptor alpha and the expression of male sociosexual behavior in two populations of prairie voles. Brain Res. 2004, 1016 (2): 247-254. 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Barron AB: Anaesthitising Drosophila for behavioural studies. J Insect Physiol. 2000, 46: 439-442. 10.1016/S0022-1910(99)00129-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Shorrocks B, Charlesworth P: The distribution and abundance of the British fungal breeding Drosophila. Ecol Entomol. 1980, 5: 61-78. 10.1111/j.1365-2311.1980.tb01124.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Wheeler NR: The insemination reaction in intraspecific matings of Drosophila. Univ Texas Publ. 1947, 4720: 78-115.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Best AR, Lewis Z, Hurst GDD, Lizé A: Thermal environment during and outside courtship jointly determine female remating rate in Drosophila melanogaster. Anim Behav. 2012, 83 (6): 1483-1490. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Ihaka R, Gentleman R: R: A language for data analysis and graphics. J Comput Graph Stat. 1996, 5 (3): 299-314.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Dr. A. Lizé for giving invaluable advice for working with D. subobscura, and Jordan Smith, Sian Davis, Gwen Cowley, Chris Shirley, Raegan McKay, Steve Parratt and Cheryl Bennett for assisting with the practical work. We thank Dr Stephen Goodwin and two anonymous referees for their comments on the paper. This work was funded by a NERC fellowship to TP (NE/H015604/1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tom A R Price.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

DF & TP conceived the remating experiments, RD & TP conceived the response to rivals experiments. DF participated in all experiments, performed statistical analysis for the remating experiments and drafted the manuscript. RD participated in all experiments, performed statistical analysis for the response to rivals experiments and drafted the manuscript. TP participated in some of the experiments, guided statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fisher, D.N., Doff, R.J. & Price, T.A.R. True polyandry and pseudopolyandry: why does a monandrous fly remate?. BMC Evol Biol 13, 157 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-13-157

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-13-157

Keywords