Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

What triggers colour change? Effects of background colour and temperature on the development of an alpine grasshopper

Abstract

Background

Colour polymorphisms are a fascinating facet of many natural populations of plants and animals, and the selective processes that maintain such variation are as relevant as the processes which promote their development. Orthoptera, the insect group that encompasses grasshoppers and bush crickets, includes a particularly large number of species that are colour polymorphic with a marked green-brown polymorphism being particularly widespread. Colour polymorphism has been associated with the need for crypsis and background matching and background-dependent homochromy has been described in a few species. However, when and how different environmental conditions influence variation in colour remains poorly understood. Here we test for effects of background colour and ambient temperature on the occurrence of colour morph switches (green to brown or brown to green) and developmental darkening in the alpine dwelling club-legged grasshopper Gomphocerus sibiricus.

Results

We monitored individually housed nymphae across three of their four developmental stages and into the first week after final ecdysis. Our data show an absence of colour morph switches in G. sibiricus, without a single switch observed in our sample. Furthermore, we test for an effect of temperature on colouration by manipulating radiant heat, a limiting factor in alpine habitats. Radiant heat had a significant effect on developmental darkening: individuals under low radiant heat tended to darken, while individuals under high radiant heat tended to lighten within nymphal stages. Young imagoes darkened under either condition.

Conclusions

Our results indicate a plastic response to a variable temperature and indicate that melanin, a multipurpose pigment responsible for dark colouration and presumed to be costly, seems to be strategically allocated according to the current environmental conditions. Unlike other orthopterans, the species is apparently unable to switch colour morphs (green/brown) during development, suggesting that colour morphs are determined genetically (or very early during development) and that other processes have to contribute to crypsis and homochromy in this species.

Background

Colour polymorphism has fascinated biologists since the time of Darwin, and its evolutionary meaning is still being revealed [13]. Colour polymorphism, defined here as within-species phenotypic variation, occurs throughout the animal kingdom in several taxa of birds, fish, mammals, frogs, molluscs, spiders, several insect orders and also in plants [49]. The occurrence of colour polymorphisms in natural populations can result from biased mutation, pleiotropy and trade-offs, gene flow, spatially and/or temporally fluctuating selection and negative frequency-dependent selection that can counter loss of variation by genetic drift [1013]. Furthermore, developmental plasticity and phenotypic flexibility, if they do not invoke significant cost, might allow the maintenance of polymorphisms. This can be particularly advantageous in unpredictably variable environments.

Insects offer a multitude of examples for the coexistence of two or more colour morphs in groups such as grasshoppers, mantoids, cicadids, damselflies, lepidopterans and beetles [1315]. There is ample evidence for genetic and environmental effects, as well as genotype-by-environment interactions in colour determination [1418]. Several species appear capable of modifying their colour in response to various environmental cues such as temperature, predation threats, behaviour stimulus (e.g., crab spiders which try to blend with their background to ambush prey, [6]), among others [19]. Within Orthoptera, colour polymorphism is present in dozens of species (reviewed in [15], see also [19, 20]). Two particularly eye-catching forms of colour polymorphism in orthopterans are a widespread green-brown polymorphism in grasshoppers and bush crickets and the famous phase polymorphism in locusts [14, 21]. Phase polymorphism is triggered by changes in population density which induces changes in colour (typically black patterns in gregarious versus pale green or brown colours in solitary phases) as part of more complex changes in morphology, physiology, behaviour and life history [2226].

The green-brown polymorphism is far more widespread among orthopterans than phase polymorphism and does not correlate with obvious changes in morphology and/or behaviour. Many families and genera of orthopterans comprise species that display either green or brown morphs, while other species are polymorphic (e.g., in genera Decticus, Metrioptera, Oedaleus). In some species, one of the morphs is very rare (such as brown morphs in Decticus verrucivorus), while in others the ratios are far more even (as in Metrioptera roeselii) [14]. With respect to environmental effects, green morphs seem to develop primarily under high humidity, while brown morphs are favoured under dry environmental conditions [15, 27, 28]. Besides the two very striking forms of colour polymorphism mentioned before, there is a range of more fine-scaled within-species variation in colour pattern and colouration among orthopterans [29, 30]. Groundhoppers, for example, differ substantially in their colour patterns, which can be categorized into variable numbers of discrete morphs [31, 32]. In other species, differences in colour are more continuous such as with colouration of species in the genus Oedipoda. Such fine-scaled variation seems to be partly under genetic, partly under environmental control [15, 16, 33]. Many species also show occasional pinkish, purple, yellow or blue colour morphs [34], further illustrating the diversity of colour in orthopterans.

A very interesting phenomenon associated with colour polymorphism is homochromy, which describes matching of body colouration with variation in the background pattern of the local habitat [14, 15, 20, 35]. Such matching might arise for four different reasons: (i) local adaptation due to multi-generational history of selection on genetic polymorphisms, (ii) selective mortality within generations, (iii) individual-level choice of matching habitat patches [36], and (iv) developmental plasticity of body colouration to match local conditions [14]. Developmental switches are particularly intriguing, because they allow individual-level matching, which is likely advantageous if habitats are unpredictably variable across generations, but predictable from environmental cues over the lifetime of individuals. Developmental matching has been reported in orthopterans for species with fine-scale variation in colour pattern [37, 38], but also for species which present the green and brown colour polymorphism (Table 1).

Table 1 Studies on the effects of background colouration on the occurrence of colour morph switches in green-brown polymorphic (upper section) and other polymorphic (lower section) orthopterans

Orthopterans are preyed upon by a large diversity of species, including birds, lizards, amphibians, spiders and other insects and are frequently parasitized by parasitic flies and mites [24, 34]. Visually hunting predators might constitute a force that can favour homochromy and crypsis, since survival to the imago stage is critical to individual fitness. Predators might also impose frequency-dependent selection if they develop search images and preferentially prey upon the most common morphs [9]. However, there are other influences that might affect body colour and this may or may not be in conflict with crypsis. For example, body colour is likely to affect the absorption of radiant heat and therefore play an important role in thermoregulation [13, 3941]. It has repeatedly been reported that orthopterans raised under cool conditions are darker than those raised under warm conditions [16, 19, 4244].

The club-legged grasshopper Gomphocerus sibiricus is a highly sexually dimorphic alpine dwelling grasshopper that exhibits the green-brown polymorphism present in many other orthopterans. Green individuals are rarer than brown morphs in most populations. Despite substantial fluctuations in population density [24, 45], the species does not show any typical phase polymorphism [26]. It inhabits alpine pastures and grassland with a very heterogeneous composition of open terrain strewn with stones and mottled by various types of short grasses and herbaceous plants. Climate conditions in the mountains are very unpredictable and variable within and between years. The maintenance of the green-brown colour polymorphism could be aided by the heterogeneous habitat and/or temporal variability in climate conditions in the native habitat of G. sibiricus.

In the present study we aimed to test the effect of two known factors on developmental colour changes in G. sibiricus. First, we assessed the effect of background colour (green or brown) on colour morph development across almost the entire ontogeny. We were particularly interested in whether individuals are able to switch colour morphs to achieve homochromy as it has been described in other species (Table 1). We predicted that if individuals were able to switch colour morphs, then individuals whose colour morph mismatched the background colour would be capable of matching their background at an advanced developmental stage. Second, we assessed the effect of temperature by means of a radiant heat treatment on developmental darkness, while controlling for humidity, population density and food moisture content. Here we predicted that if individuals were capable of manipulate the degree of melanin in their cuticle, thermoregulation needs would promote a colouration darkening under conditions of low radiation. We followed individuals from the second nymphal stage through to the imaginal stage during two independent rounds of trials with two different radiant heat regimes. Individuals were exposed to experimental treatments from the second nymphal stage onwards. The long exposure to experimental conditions allowed us to evaluate if colour changes occur exclusively in connection with moults or if changes were possible even within nymphal stages.

Results

A total of 78 individuals survived the first nymphal stage and entered the experimental setup in two rounds (referred to as R1 and R2). From this total, 34 individuals matched their cage background, while 44 did not match. Sixty one individuals were brown (78 %, 36 in R1 and 25 in R2) and 17 green (22 %, 6 in R1 and 11 in R2). The distribution of morphs in the R2 differed significantly between the sexes with brown individuals of both sexes (11 males = 44 %, 14 females = 56 %), but only green females (11 individuals) (Χ 2 = 5.05, df = 1, p = 0.025).

Colour morph switches

No colour morph switch was observed among the 34 individuals in cages with matched background colours (6 green individuals in green cages and 28 brown in brown cages in total for both rounds). Forty-four individuals (33 brown and 11 green) were exposed to unmatched backgrounds, but no colour morph switches were observed among these 44 individuals. Reasoning based on binomial sampling (see methods section) suggests that if G. sibiricus is capable of switch colour, the rate of colour morph switches is well below values reported in previous studies (c ≤ 0.07, Table 2). When we concentrate on the subset of the data where colour morph switches were most likely, given both non-matched background and temperature treatment (brown individuals on a green background under high radiant heat treatment), the probability of colour morph switch is still well below expectations (c ≤ 0.17, Table 2).

Table 2 Number of individuals of G. sibiricus on mismatched backgrounds, all of which did not switch colour morph during development

Temperature-cued colouration darkening

We observed a significant change in colouration darkness in both rounds of trials. Individuals in the second and third nymphal stages (N2 and N3) of the round 1, and in the N2 and N4 stages of the round 2 experienced a change in darkness which depended on the direction of the temperature treatment. Individuals in the low radiant heat treatment became darker in colour, while those in the high radiant heat treatment became lighter in colour (Table 3, Fig. 1). Individuals in the N3 stage in the R2 did not show a significant change in colour, yet the sign of the point estimate is the same as in stages N2 and N4. Individuals in the N4 stage of the R1 and under a high radiant heat treatment got significantly darker with age. Most of the low radiant heat treatment individuals from the R1 had perished, with the single remaining individual becoming lighter. Unlike the situation in nymphal stages, individuals in the imago stage undergo a darkening in colour in both treatments within the first week after final ecdysis (Fig. 1).

Table 3 Results of the random-slope mixed effects model used to test for effects of radiant heat treatment and larval age on colouration darkness in both green and brown coloured morphs
Fig. 1
figure 1

Changes in darkness across nymphal stages exposed to two different sets of radiant heat regimes (first round R1, and second round R2). In each set of panels, upper panels show low radiant heat treatments, bottom panels show high radiant heat treatments. Each line represents the trajectory of an individual (some individuals have only one observation per nymphal stage, and therefore appear only as a dot without line). Brown lines represent brown morphs, while green lines represent green morphs. Nymphal stages are measured in days since the start of each nymphal stage. Darkness has four levels, with 1 representing the lightest colour morph and 4 representing the darkest colour morph

The analysis of unambiguous cases of lightening or darkening using Fisher’s exact tests confirmed a significant colour change for the N2 and N4 stages in the R2 (N2 stage: n = 14, p = 0.023; N3 stage: n = 8, p = 0.14; N4 stage: n = 10, p = 0.015; IM stage: n = 12, p = 0.47, see also Fig. 1). Yet after exclusion of ambiguous cases, there was not significant effect in the R1 (N2 stage: n = 12, p = 0.091; N3 stage: n = 7, p = 0.28; N4 stage: n = 12, p = 0.33, see also Fig. 1).

The comparison of treatments per observation time (early or late observations within nymphal stages) also confirmed a significant effect of the treatment in the colour of nymphae (upper p values in Fig. 2). Individuals in the N2 stage in both rounds, and in the N3 stage of R1 show a significant difference in colouration darkness at the end of the nymphal stage. Individuals of the N4 stage differ at the start of the nymphal stage, but at the end colouration darkness had converged.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean darkness per nymphal stage at the earliest and latest observation point, separated per radiant heat treatment for two different sets of radiant heat regimes. Upper panel shows the first round (R1), lower panel shows the second round (R2). White bars represent low radiant heat treatments, grey bars represent high radiant heat treatments. Numbers inside the bar show the number of individuals per observation point. P-values for two-sample t-tests comparing treatments within observation points are shown on top (labeled as “Early” and “Late”). P-values for paired t-tests comparing observation points within treatments are shown below (labeled as “High” and “Low”)

Finally the comparison between the earliest and latest observations per treatment within nymphal stages also found a significant effect of the treatment in the colouration darkness of nymphae (lower p values in Fig. 2). Individuals in the N2 stage of both rounds (statistically significant only in the low radiant heat treatment), N3 stage of R1 (statistically significant only in the high radiant heat treatment) and N4 stages of R2 change their colouration darkness within nymphal stages. The colour change in the IM stage is also significant and the change in both groups is towards a darker coloration.

Discussion

We here show the effect of background colour and temperature on colour changes and darkening in G. sibiricus. There were no colour morph switches among 78 individuals tested, neither when exposed to matched nor to unmatched background. Colour morph switches have been widely reported in other orthopterans (Table 1), hence it is interesting that we cannot confirm this for our species. If this species is capable of switching colour at all, the probability of colour morph switch in individuals that mismatched their cage background was certainly very low for both rounds of trials (≤7 %). Additionally, we find that the amount of radiant heat affected colouration darkening within nymphal stages, with darkening at low amounts of radiant heat and lightening at high amounts of radiant heat. Imagoes tended to darken within the first week after final ecdysis independent of radiant heat treatment, suggesting that imagoes may face different life-history trade-offs than nymphae.

Green-brown switches

Colour morph switches have been previously reported, with several observations of the phenomenon in a diverse array of species (Table 1). Several of the species for which colour morph switches have been reported are members of the Acrididae (the family that also includes Gomphocerus), but none of them is a member of the subfamily Gomphocerinae [46]. It is possible that the capability for colour morph switches has been lost somewhere in the branch of Gomphocerinae, but this remains speculative in the absence of information about other species. The lack of a mechanism for switching colour during nymphal stages in G. sibiricus may also be due to the really fine-grained structure of the habitat inhabited by the species. It would be very costly for an individual to move at all within the matrix of colours of the habitat if this would require an active colour switching to match their background, even if this could be done in a relatively short time window.

Background colour was visually perceivable to the developing individuals and based on previous studies, we assume visual perception to be the main input that triggers colour change to match the background (see [14] and references therein). However, it might be argued that our coloured paper was not of the right kind for triggering colour changes. Previous studies have used a large diversity of materials for the background manipulation, such as stones, sawdust, sand, coal, clay, paper and paint (Table 1). These different materials have typically elicited colour morph switches, which suggests that the effect does not depend on the exact kind of materials used as background. We consider it unlikely (albeit possible) that our paper type was so substantially different from previously used materials that it would not be suitable for triggering colour switches.

In previous experiments, individuals have been tested for different periods of time in order to assess colour morph switches, usually starting the experiments at early nymphal stages [27, 32, 4749]. Colour morph switches have typically been reported to occur across nymphal stages, often quantified a few days after ecdysis, though detailed information on the exact timing of switches is usually lacking. The only exception to colour morph changes occurring within nymphal stages are changes to black colouration, which have been reported to occur during the imaginal stage [14]. We started with our experimental treatment very early in the life of the grasshoppers (also in comparison with previous studies), giving scope for switches within and/or between developmental stages. Therefore it is rather unlikely that the duration of the treatment prevented colour switching.

Switches might also have been expected due to the radiant heat treatment, since brown individuals tend to be darker on average than green individuals and we would expect them to be better able to heat up if radiant heat is limiting. High temperature, as well as high humidity, high food moisture content and low individual density, are known to drive green body colouration in grasshoppers [14, 15]. We expect that in our experiment high radiant heat conditions would have served to cue individuals of a green habitat. High alpine habitats are typically characterized by high humidity regimes due to high condensation of air humidity at night, which is available as dew drops early in the mornings, and also due to high precipitation regimes [50]. Under these conditions, high temperature and high humidity will promote vegetation growth and produce greener habitats than conditions of low temperature, where vegetation would have weaker growth. Such conditions could have promoted colour morph switches from brown to green, possibly as a means for habitat matching. In contrast, low radiant heat conditions would have served to cue individuals of a browner habitat with less flourishing vegetation. These conditions, in the case of green individuals, could have promoted colour morph switches from green to brown, either for habitat matching and/or to improve thermoregulatory capacity.

Green individuals on a brown background and under the low radiant heat treatment thus constitute the subgroup for which colour morphs switches from green to brown appear particularly advantageous. It is possible that the combination of relative high humidity, high food moisture content and individual housing (i.e., low population density) counteracted the effect of the background, hampering the occurrence of the colour morph switch [14, 15]. This is different for brown individuals on a green background and under the high radiant heat treatment, since for those individuals the combination of low density, high humidity, background mismatching and no need for improved heat absorption are all expected to favour colour morph switches towards green. Yet none of the 16 individuals under this suit of conditions switched colour.

Our limited sample size does not allow us to exclude the possibility that G. sibiricus is capable of colour morph switches under some conditions. Still it strongly points against frequent, general developmental switches in response to background colour and temperature. We had expected that if colour morph switches occurred in G. sibiricus, they would occur at the nymphal stages, given that matching the habitat background is expected to improve survival in natural conditions. It is possible that nymphs of G. sibiricus achieve homochromy even in the absence of developmental switches by actively seeking out matching (micro)habitats [36]. Habitats of G. sibiricus are spatially highly heterogeneous and this might distinguish them from many of the other species that show developmental colour morph switches. Microhabitat variability might favour behavioural over developmental homochromy, while more global (temporal) variability in less structured habitats might favour developmental switches.

Colouration darkening

While the temperature treatment in our experiment did not elicit colour morph switches, it elicited a more subtle response in colouration, causing darkening under low radiant and lightening under high radiant conditions. Radiant heat can limit behaviour of individuals by hampering thermoregulation and this in turn can constrain activity levels, growth and development, and ultimately fitness [5153]. An increase in the amount of melanin under the cuticle surface would improve thermoregulatory capability due to a difference in heat absorbance between black and brown or green colours, and this would help counteract the effect of a short window of radiant heat exposure. Hence the darkening that we found in the low heat treatment is in line with what would be expected for improved thermoregulation [3941].

The lightening in colouration in response to the high radiant heat treatment can result from a trade-off between melanin as a colour pigment and other functions of melanin. Melanin plays a role in several functions in insects, such as immune defence, integumental colouration, wound healing and cuticle sclerotisation, among others (reviewed in [54]). It has also been documented that melanin production in insects can be costly, mostly because of the many possible functions of melanin, but also because of dietary limitations of melanin precursors or lack of enzymes necessary to process precursors [51, 5557]. Therefore lightening of colouration can be seen as an option to avoid investing melanin in body colouration when it is not necessary for absorbing more radiant heat. This reasoning might give an adaptive explanation for the lightening in our high radiant heat treatments.

A change in darkness within nymphal stages implies a mechanism which allows individuals to adjust the amount of visible melanin in their epidermis during the relatively short time spanned between moults. Such a mechanism would include cells at the epidermis capable of spreading pigment granules under the cuticle surface, but also capable of withdrawing the pigment granules under proper stimulation [19, 58]. Relatively little is known about the physiology of colour changes in grasshoppers, but different physiological and morphological mechanisms have been described in other arthropods.

A very intuitive mechanism which could explain the changes in darkness under different radiant heat regimes is pigment dispersal and concentration within chromatophores [19, 59]. This type of cell is known to be present in several taxa, such as fish, reptiles, amphibians, crustaceans and bacteria [60]. The shape of chromatophores is typically highly branched, allowing for pigment to disperse to the branches or contract to the centre to achieve colour change [19, 59, 61]. Another physiological mechanism involved in colour change is granule migration. Here granules of pigment are transported along microtubules which are perpendicular to the cuticular surface, and which branch distally, allowing the pigments to spread and therefore causing colour change. A striking example of this plastic mechanism is observed in the temperature-controlled daily changes in the colour of the chameleon grasshopper Kosciuscola tristis. In this case granules of pigment migrate from the epidermis when the grasshopper is exposed to temperatures above 25 °C, giving males a bright turquoise colouration [19]. A similar mechanism is used in the stick insect Carausius morosus [62]. The colour change that we found in G. sibiricus is much slower, but since granule migration might simultaneously explain changes in darkness in both directions, it might contribute to our observations.

The unpredictable climate in the habitat of G. sibiricus, characterized by long spans of time with few favourable climatic conditions, might explain the occurrence of a darkening and lightening mechanism. In this environment, given the duration of each nymphal stage (about one week, albeit likely to be substantially longer in the field) nymphae may need to adjust colouration even within nymphal stages to be able to cope with climatic variability. In species dwelling in habitats where radiant heat is a limited factor, energy balance and thus activity levels during early developmental stages could be hampered by limited sun exposure conditions. Being able to adjust colouration darkness could greatly improve the use of resources by an individual, in this case melanin which is a multipurpose and apparently costly pigment [63].

Conclusions

Colour polymorphism is a striking feature of several species, which involves variation in the types of morphs and different environmental triggers and genetic determinants which combine to help define the colour morph of an individual [14, 38]. Here we report the absence of colour morph switches in the alpine grasshopper G. sibiricus. The lack of switches could be driven by the heterogeneity of the habitat of G. sibiricus. Additionally we found that nymphae of G. sibiricus are capable of modifying the amount of colouration darkness in response to radiant heat. This capability could help nymphae adjust colouration within nymphal stages according to the unstable climatic conditions characteristic of their habitat, and in turn improve allocation of pigment resources to other physiological processes. The complex net of interactions between fine-scaled variation in polymorphisms, unpredictable and highly variable environments, and melanin pathways which may be costly and also shared by different developmental processes are not only a very promising avenue of study in behavioural ecology, but they will also change the way in which we assess colour polymorphism in natural populations.

Methods

Subjects

Last-instar individuals of G. sibiricus were collected in the field (near Sierre, Valais, Switzerland) in July 2013 and brought to the laboratory where they moulted into imagoes. We housed imagoes in separate cages (dimensions 22 × 16 × 16 cm3) containing one male and one female and provided a cup of sand-vermiculite mixture as substrate for egg laying. Eggs were collected once per week, kept for approximately 6 weeks at room temperature and subsequently stored at 4 °C for diapause. After seven months in the refrigerator, eggs were taken out of their diapause and kept at room temperature until hatching. In total, 116 hatchlings hatched from 37 egg pods. Full-siblings from the same egg pod were housed together in the same cage (white cages lined with black mesh, dimensions as above) throughout the first nymphal stage, but were separated after the first nymphal stage (i.e., after about one week) and thereafter housed individually. We refer to the different nymphal stages as N2 (second nymphal stage), N3, N4 and IM (imago stage).

Experimental setup

The experiments were conducted under artificial full-spectral light conditions (Biolux L 58 W/965, OSRAM, Munich, Germany) with a 14:10 H light dark cycle (7:00 till 21:00). Average humidity was kept at 70 % by humidifiers. In order to maintain a high moisture content in the food, we provided fresh grass in small plastic vials (height x diameter: 5.8 cm × 2.1 cm) filled with water, and replaced the grass every other day in order to avoid withered or yellowing grass. Experiments were conducted in two rounds (we refer to the two rounds as R1 and R2) that differed in details of the radiant heat treatment, because of high nymphal mortality under one of the conditions in R1. Experimental setups consisted of blocks of four cages (11 and 10 replicate blocks in the R1 and R2, respectively) flanked on both sides by isolating dividers (22 cm × 55 cm × 1 cm) (Fig. 3). The dividers were installed to isolate alternating blocks exposed to different experimental conditions. Individuals were transferred to experimental setups on the day of moulting into the N2 and assignment to treatments was done at random. All individuals remained under the same experimental conditions until one week after moulting into IM (unless they died before completing the experimental period). Only three individuals reached the last nymphal stage under low radiant heat conditions in R1, two of which died shortly after, and therefore the experiment was stopped at this point due to lack of one treatment group. We did not score the sex of individuals during early nymphal stages because this is easily visible during the N4 stage. In the case of the R1, individuals died rapidly while the experiment was running, and sex scoring was not assessed in time. For this reason data on colour morph frequencies for both sexes is not presented in the results.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Experimental setup. Two blocks are shown, one exposed to the high radiant heat (orange dot) and one to the low radiant heat (yellow dot) treatment. Blocks are separated by dividers (Di). Each cage has its floor and two of its sides lined in green or brown paper, and each block has two cages lined with each colour. Time of exposure to radiant heat varied between rounds (see details in Temperature treatment section). The doors (do) on the sides allow easy access to cages

Background colour treatment

The first component of our experimental design involved an assessment of homochromy. Specifically we tested for changes in colour morph from green to brown or brown to green in response to differences in background colour. Each cage (same type as described above) was lined on two of its inner sides as well as the cage’s floor with coloured paper (Coloured drawing paper, green = #ADFF2F, brown = #8B4513, Folia Paper Bringmann, Wendelstein, Germany). Each experimental block consisted of two cages lined with brown colour and two cages lined with green colour. Individuals had visual contact to only one neighbouring cage that was ensured to be of the same colour as their own cages (Fig. 3).

Temperature treatment

The second component of our experimental setup consisted of a temperature treatment and was applied to entire blocks. Grasshoppers regulate their body temperature behaviourally, elevating it substantially above ambient temperature by sun-bathing [24]. We aimed to simulate high (sunny) and low (overcast) radiant heat conditions, both of which occur in the natural habitats of G. sibiricus. Therefore, we placed 150 W infra-red heat bulbs (IOT/90, Elstein, Northeim, Germany) at the vortex of each block. In the R1, blocks were exposed to either 5 h (high treatment) or 1 h (low treatment) of radiant heat, but due to high mortality, this was increased to 4 + 6 h (high treatment) or 2 h (low treatment) per day in the R2 (Fig. 3). The hours of exposure in the R1 for the high treatment were from 9:00 until 12:00, and again from 13:00 until 15:00, while the exposure time for the low treatment was from 12:00 until 13:00. The hours of exposure in the R2 for the high treatment were from 8:00 until 12:00, and again from 14:00 until 20:00, while the exposure time for the low treatment was from 12:00 until 14:00. The temperature at exposed locations was around 44 °C when heat lamps were active, in contrast to the 25 °C when heat lamps were switched off. In the high radiant heat treatment, individuals were able to adjust the amount of radiant heat that they received by positing themselves at the mesh ceiling or by moving away from it or also by hiding behind the beams of the cage.

Scoring of body colour

Colour morphs are identifiable from the third day of the second nymphal stage. Both morphs vary in the degree of colouration darkness (e.g., from very light green, across a discreet range of green tones, to very dark green morphs). Pale versions (which presumably have reduced bile pigments, ommochromes and melanin – responsible for green, brown and black colouration, respectively; [6, 35]) as well as melanic versions (very dark in colouration) occur in the lab and in the field. We defined two intermediate categories among those extremes, based on a collection of photographs of individuals taken either in the lab or in the field on previous years. The scoring was done every third day, assigning the individuals a colour grade based on the degree of darkness in the area under study. The categories where defined as follows: 1. Very light = < 10 % of black; 2. Light = 30-40 % of black; 3. Dark = 60-70 % of black; 4. Very dark = > 90 % of black. Our analysis is based on a total of 433 scorings from the R1 and 306 scorings from the R2.

Statistical analysis

Since our sample size is necessarily finite, it is at least possible that despite an absence in our sample, colour morph switches occur with a low probability. We therefore estimated the lowest probability of colour morph switches that would still be consistent with our data. We used the following approach to assess the highest colour change probability that is consistent with our data. We define c as the probability of colour change in unmatched backgrounds, (1-c) as the probability of no change and k as the number of individuals exposed to unmatched backgrounds. Based on binomial sampling, we expect to observe no colour morph switches among k individuals with a probability of (1-c)k. We defined a probability threshold of α = 0.05 and searched for the value of c at which the expectation of no-colour morph switches is equal to α. This gives the highest possible switching probability that is still considered reasonably consistent with our data.

To test for the effect of the temperature treatment in the body colour we used a random-slope mixed effects model. We chose this model because the possible response to the treatment could vary physiologically from individual to individual, making it necessary to account for this variability in our analysis [64]. The p-values for the fixed effects in the mixed models were approximated by a t distribution with degrees of freedom equal to the number of individuals minus the number of fixed effect parameters estimated in the model. This approach is conservative in that it considers only individuals as independent datapoints. However, such random-slope models depend on assumptions about the linearity of the response-covariance relationship and about the distributions of random-deviations from the population mean and population slope. Therefore, we applied additional, simpler tests for the same question in order to verify the robustness of the results (see below for details).

In addition to the analysis of the data using random slope models, we considered using alternative ways of analysis to confirm the robustness of the results on colouration darkening. First, we identified those individuals who showed an unambiguous lightening or darkening in colour (individuals with bidirectional changes in colour darkening within a nymphal or imago stage were left out: R1, n = 10 nymphae + 1 imago; R2, n = 2 imagoes). Then we counted the number of cases of darkening and lightening in each stage, and compared them using Fisher’s exact tests. Second, we compared treatments per observation time (early or late observations within nymphal stages) between treatments using two sample t-tests. Third, we compared the darkness between the earliest and latest observations per treatment within nymphal stages using paired t-tests. This was only possible for those individuals that had at least two observations per nymphal stage. All analysis were conducted in R 3.1.1 [65], and we used the lme4 package [66] for mixed model fitting.

Availability of supporting data

The data sets supporting the results of this article are available in the Dryad repository: doi:10.5061/dryad.j95t2 [67].

Abbreviations

N1:

First nymphal stage

N2:

Second nymphal stage

N3:

Third nymphal stage

N4:

Fourth nymphal stage

IM:

Imaginal stage

R1:

First round

R2:

Second round

References

  1. Fisher RA. The evolution of dominance in certain polymorphic species. Am Nat. 1930;64:385–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Darwin C. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: J. Murray; 1871.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Mayr E. Systematics and the origin of species, from the viewpoint of a zoologist. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1942.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Darwin C. On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: J. Murray; 1859.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  5. Whiteley DAA, Owen DF, Smith DAS. Massive polymorphism and natural selection in Donacilla cornea (Poli, 1791) (Bivalvia: Mesodesmatidae). Biol J Linn Soc. 1997;62(4):475–94.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Oxford GS, Gillespie RG. Evolution and ecology of spider coloration. Annu Rev Entomol. 1998;43(1):619–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Hoffman EA, Blouin MS. A review of colour and pattern polymorphisms in anurans. Biol J Linn Soc. 2000;70(4):633–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Mundy NI. A window on the genetics of evolution: MC1R and plumage colouration in birds. Proc R Soc B. 2005;272(1573):1633–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bond AB. The evolution of color polymorphism: crypticity, searching images, and apostatic selection. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2007;38:489–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gray SM, McKinnon JS. Linking color polymorphism maintenance and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol. 2006;22(2):71–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Punzalan D, Rodd FH, Hughes KA. Perceptual processes and the maintenance of polymorphism through frequency-dependent predation. Evol Ecol. 2005;19(3):303–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. McKinnon JS, Pierotti MER. Colour polymorphism and correlated characters: genetic mechanisms and evolution. Mol Ecol. 2010;19(23):5101–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Majerus MEN. Melanism: Evolution in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Rowell CHF. The variable coloration of the acridoid grasshoppers. Advances in Insect Physiology. 1971;8:145–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Dearn JM. Color pattern polymorphism. In: Chapman RF, Joern A, editors. In: Biology of grasshoppers. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1990. p. 517–49.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Key KHL. The taxonomy, phases, and distribution of the genera Chortoicetes Brunn. and Austroicetes UY (Orthoptera: Acrididae). C.S.I.R.O.: Canberra; 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Bellmann H, Luquet CH. Guide des sauterelles, grillons et criquets d'Europe occidentale. Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bellmann H. Der Kosmos Heuschreckenführer. Stuttgart: Franckh-Kosmos-Verlag; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Umbers KDL, Fabricant SA, Gawryszewski FM, Seago AE, Herberstein ME. Reversible colour change in Arthropoda. Biol Rev. 2014;89(4):820–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Booth CL. Evolutionary significance of ontogenetic colour change in animals. Biol J Linn Soc. 1990;40(2):125–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Song H. Phylogenetic perspectives on the evolution of locust phase polyphenism. J Orthop Res. 2005;14(2):235–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Pener MP, Yerushalmi Y. The physiology of locust phase polymorphism: an update. J Insect Physiol. 1998;44(5–6):365–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Simpson SJ, Sword GA. Locusts. Curr Biol. 2008;18(9):R364–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Uvarov BP. Grasshoppers and locusts: A handbook of general acridology. London and New York: Cambridge University Press; 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Pener MP. Locust phase polymorphism and its endocrine relations. Advances in Insect Physiology. 1991;23:1–79.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Song H. Density-dependent phase polyphenism in nonmodel locusts: A minireview. Psyche. 2011;2011:1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Rowell CHF, Cannis TL. Environmental factors affecting the green/brown polymorphism in the Cyrtacanthacridine grasshopper Schistocerca vaga (Scudder). Acrida. 1971;1(1):69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Lecoq M, Pierozzi Jr I. Chromatic polymorphism and geophagy: Two outstanding characteristics of Rhammatocerus schistocercoides (Rehn 1906) grasshoppers in Brazil (Orthoptera, Acrididae, Gomphocerinae). J Orthop Res. 1996;5:13–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Richards OW, Waloff N. Studies on the biology and population dynamics of British grasshoppers, vol. 17. London: Anti-locust Research Centre; 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Rubtzov IA. Phase variation in non-swarming grasshoppers. Bull Entomol Res. 1935;26(04):499–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Nabours R. The genetics of the Tettigidae (grouse locusts). Netherlands: Springer; 1929.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  32. Forsman A, Ringblom K, Civantos E, Ahnesjö J. Coevolution of color pattern and thermoregulatory behavior in polymorphic pygmy grasshoppers Tetrix undulata. Evolution. 2002;56(2):349–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ergene S. Weitere Untersuchungen über Farbanpassung bei Oedaleus decorus. Z Vgl Physiol. 1955;37(3):226–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Ingrisch S, Köhler G. Die Heuschrecken Mitteleuropas. Westarp Wissenschaften: Magdeburg; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Fuzeau-Braesch S. Pigments and color changes. Annu Rev Entomol. 1972;17:403–24.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Edelaar P, Siepielski AM, Clobert J. Matching habitat choice causes directed gene flow: A neglected dimension in evolution and ecology. Evolution. 2008;62(10):2462–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Ergene S. Homochrome Farbanpassung bei Oedipoda Larven. Z Vgl Physiol. 1952;34(2):163–5.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Karlsson M, Caesar S, Ahnesjö J, Forsman A. Dynamics of colour polymorphism in a changing environment: fire melanism and then what? Oecologia. 2008;154(4):715–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Hegna RH, Nokelainen O, Hegna JR, Mappes J. To quiver or to shiver: increased melanization benefits thermoregulation, but reduces warning signal efficacy in the wood tiger moth. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280(1755):20122812.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Forsman A. Some like it hot: Intra-population variation in behavioral thermoregulation in color-polymorphic pygmy grasshoppers. Evol Ecol. 2000;14(1):25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Kingsolver JG, Wiernasz DC. Seasonal polyphenism in wing-melanin pattern and thermoregulatory adaptation in Pieris butterflies. Am Nat. 1991;137(6):816–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Okay S. The effect of temperature and humidity on the formation of green pigment in Acrida bicolor (Thunb). Arch Int Physiol Biochim. 1956;64(1):80–91.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Duck LG. The bionomics of Schistocerca obscura (Fabr). J Kansas Entomol Soc. 1944;17(3):105–19.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Abushama FT. The effect of light on the dispersal of ‘yellow’ melanin pigments in the integument of the grasshopper Poecilocerus hieroglyphicus (Klug.). Experientia. 1969;25(3):326–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Sergeev MG, Kopaneva LM, Rubtsov IA, Antipanova EM, Burgov AG, Vysotskaya LV, et al. Siberian locust (Aeropus sibiricus L.) [Sibirskaya kobylka (Aeropus sibiricus L.)]. Russian Akademia NAUK: Novosibirsk; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Zhang HL, Huang Y, Lin LL, Wang XY, Zheng ZM. The phylogeny of the Orthoptera (Insecta) as deduced from mitogenomic gene sequences. Zool Stud. 2013;52(1):1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Tanaka S. Environmental control of body-color polyphenism in the American grasshopper, Schistocerca americana. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2004;97(2):293–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Ergene S. Untersuchungen über Farbanpassung und Farbwechsel bei Acrida turrita. Z Vgl Physiol. 1950;32(6):530–51.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Hochkirch A, Deppermann J, Groning J. Phenotypic plasticity in insects: the effects of substrate color on the coloration of two ground-hopper species. Evol Dev. 2008;10(3):350–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Beniston M. Mountain weather and climate: A general overview and a focus on climatic change in the Alps. Hydrobiologia. 2006;562:3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Ellers J, Boggs CL. Functional ecological implications of intraspecific differences in wing melanization in Colias butterflies. Biol J Linn Soc. 2004;82(1):79–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. May ML. Insect thermoregulation. Annu Rev Entomol. 1979;24:313–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Willott SJ, Hassall M. Life-history responses of British grasshoppers (Orthoptera : Acrididae) to temperature change. Funct Ecol. 1998;12(2):232–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sugumaran H. Comparative biochemistry of eumelanogenesis and the protective roles of phenoloxidase and melanin in insects. Pigment Cell Res. 2002;15(1):2–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Stoehr AM. Costly melanin ornaments: the importance of taxon? Funct Ecol. 2006;20(2):276–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Stoehr AM. Responses of disparate phenotypically-plastic, melanin-based traits to common cues: limits to the benefits of adaptive plasticity? Evol Ecol. 2010;24(2):287–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Roff DA, Fairbairn DJ. The costs of being dark: the genetic basis of melanism and its association with fitness-related traits in the sand cricket. J Evol Biol. 2013;26(7):1406–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Chapman RF. The Insects: Structure and Function: Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK.1998.

  59. Fingerman M. Comparative physiology: Chromatophores. Annu Rev Physiol. 1970;32(1):345–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Fingerman M. Chromatophores. Physiol Rev. 1965;45:296–339.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Keeling PJ, Archibald JM. Organelle evolution: What's in a name? Curr Biol. 2008;18(8):R345–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Berthold G. Microtubules in the epidermal cells of Carausius morosus (Br) - Their pattern and relation to pigment migration. J Insect Physiol. 1980;26(6):421–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Burtt EH. The adaptiveness of animal colors. Bioscience. 1981;31(10):723–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W. Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behav Ecol. 2009;20(2):416–20.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Core Team R. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B: lme4. Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. 2012, R package version 0.999999-0. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.

  67. Valverde P, Schielzeth H. Data from: What Triggers Colour Change? Background Colour and Temperature Effects on the Development of an Alpine Grasshopper. Dryad Digital Repository. doi:10.5061/dryad.j95t2.

  68. Tanaka S, Harano K, Nishide Y. Re-examination of the roles of environmental factors in the control of body-color polyphenism in solitarious nymphs of the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria with special reference to substrate color and humidity. J Insect Physiol. 2012;58(1):89–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hertz M, Imms AD. On the responses of the african migratory locust to different types of background. Proc R Soc B. 1937;122(828):281–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Helfert B. Environmental effects on color variation and ommochromes in Chorthippus biguttulus L. (Orthoptera, Acrididae). Zool Anz. 1978;200(1–2):41–52.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Amy Backhouse for the valuable feedback and additional help provided. Thanks to Bahar Patlar and Petra Dieker for valuable comments on the manuscript. Thanks to Axel Hochkirch for the data provided for the polymorphism table. Thanks to Veronica Dahm for her valuable comments and support. The project was funded by an Emmy Noether grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG; SCHI 1188/1-1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Pablo Valverde.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JPV and HS perceived and designed the experiment. JPV performed the experiments and drafted the manuscript. JPV and HS analysed the data and the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Valverde, J.P., Schielzeth, H. What triggers colour change? Effects of background colour and temperature on the development of an alpine grasshopper. BMC Evol Biol 15, 168 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0419-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0419-9

Keywords