Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Allopatry, competitor recognition and heterospecific aggression in crater lake cichlids

Abstract

Background

Aggressive behaviour can have significant evolutionary consequences–not only within species, but also in the context of heterospecific interactions. Here, we carried out an experimental field study to investigate the importance of phenotypic similarity on levels of aggression between species whilst controlling for familiarity effects using manipulated allopatric stimuli. Specifically, we investigated aggressive responses of territory holding males and females in two species of Neotropical cichlid fish, Amphilophus sagittae and Hypsophrys nicaraguensis, that differ in their phenotypic similarity to our allopatric stimulus species, Amphilophus astorquii.

Results

We found that, independent of phenotypic similarity (and correlated phylogenetic proximity) between the territory holders and intruder, territorial aggression was not adjusted in relation to allopatric intruder colour markings that are associated with different levels of threat and known to provoke different responses in a sympatric setting. We also found that males and females did not differ in their overall patterns of aggression adjustment towards intruder cues. Nevertheless, the two focal species, which share the same breeding grounds and external threats, exhibited different sex roles in breeding territory defence.

Conclusion

Together with earlier studies assessing hetrospecific aggression in sympatry, our current results highlight the importance of coevolution and learning in species interactions.

Background

Heterospecific aggression tends to be more pronounced among congeneric, phenotypically similar species as compared to aggression directed to those from other genera and different phenotypes [1, 2]. Besides the extent of niche overlap, competitor recognition is assumed to play a significant role in the evolution of heterospecific aggression [1, 2]. Indeed, to properly adjust its aggressive responses, a territory holder needs to be able to correctly recognize (heterospecific) intruders that pose different levels of threat [3, 4]. Here, individuals may rely on similar sensory and cognitive means for recognising phenotypically similar heterospecifics as they would for conspecifics [5]. As a consequence, it may be easier for territory holders to appropriately adjust aggression towards those heterospecifics to which they have a higher phenotypic resemblance. Likewise, a novel heterospecific signal may be easier to detect when it is similar to a familiar signal [6].

Signal recognition can be driven not only by niche overlap or phenotypic resemblance between interacting species [2, 6, 7], but also by learning opportunities. For example, blue-coloured males in Pundamilia cichlid fish adjust their aggression depending on their prior exposure to red (as opposed to only blue) males [8]. Such effects can also be sex-specific, as shown in female damselflies, with species recognition in a mating context being affected by their prior experience with conspecific and heterospecific males [9]. The result of a recent meta-analysis also suggests that species recognition, at least in a mating context, may have evolved quite differently between the sexes, with the capacity to discriminate between conspecifics and heterospecifcs being based more on learning in females [5]. Consistent with such an interpretation, the sexes are often subject to divergent selection pressures in terms of, for example, aggressive behaviour and parental roles [10, 11] while, at the proximate level, they may also differ in cognitive abilities in colour or pattern recognition [12, 13]. Based on such findings, it is conceivable that the sexes could also differ both in their opportunity and ability to recognise territorial intruders, a prediction that has hitherto been subject to very little empirical attention [5].

In fish, colour cues have often evolved to play an important role in species recognition, in both a competition and reproduction context. This is especially true in cichlids [14, 15]. For instance, in the Central American Midas cichlid species complex (within the genus Amphilophus, see [16, 17]), experiments with manipulated (i.e. ‘dummy’) stimuli have found that coloration alone is a sufficient cue for competitor recognition both within [18] and among sympatric species [19]. Earlier results on African cichlids also indicate that phenotypic similarity may affect heterospecific aggression at the species (or morph) level [20, 21]. However, we currently know far less about whether individuals, when reacting to another species, are capable of adjusting their aggression according to differences in threat levels posed by different individuals of that species [22], or how familiarity or opportunities for learning may affect the adjustment of such aggression [5]. Accordingly, in a field-based experiment, we tested the influence of phenotypic similarity on aggression, as directed by breeding territory holders towards heterospecific ‘intruders’ in cichlids living in Nicaraguan Crater Lake Xiloá. Our study focused on two species of territory holders, Amphilophus sagittae and Hypsophrys nicaraguensis, which differed in their phenotypic similarity to the allopatric intruder species, Amphilophus astorquii, with which they were presented. The intruder species used in our study is allopatric with the two focal species, allowing us to control for any behavioural differences that might arise due to prior experience with the stimulus.

When ready to spawn, pairs of these cichlid fish species claim a sedentary breeding territory, which they aggressively defend for approximately a month after their fry have become free-swimming [2326]. This aggression is directed towards both competitors (especially for territory space) and brood predators that can be conspecific, congeneric, as well as more distantly related species [25, 27]. In this respect, not all intruders pose the same level of threat. For instance, breeding individuals are likely to represent a lower threat than non-breeders, as the former have already claimed, and are busy defending, a territory and offspring of their own (and relying on previously accumulated energy reserves to do so), instead of actively seeking prey. In contrast, non-breeding individuals are much more likely to attempt to prey upon eggs and juveniles of both conspecifics and heterospecifics [22, 25, 26, 28]. Supporting this scenario, an earlier observational study of fish in Crater Lake Apoyo (Nicaragua) showed that non-breeding A. astorquii are subjected to more intense aggression than breeders, by both conspecific and congeneric (Amphilophus zaliosus) territory holders [22]. Importantly, breeding and non-breeding individuals (both males and females) of A. astorquii–as well as those of A. sagittae and A. xiloaensis in Crater Lake Xiloá–have strikingly different body markings: in contrast to the uniformly dark colour of the breeders, non-breeders have contrasting dark and light vertical bars along their flanks ([22]; Figs. 1 and 2). In addition, another recent study shows that for A. sagittae territory holders, colour patterning alone is a sufficient cue for directing more aggression towards model intruders with non-breeder colour markings than with breeder coloration, when these are look-alikes of the sympatric and congeneric species A. xiloaensis [19]. In contrast to the above-mentioned Amphilophus species, our other focal territorial species, H. nicaraguensis, does not exhibit any clear differences in body markings between breeding and non-breeding phases. It does, however, share the breeding habitat with multiple Amphilophus species (including A. sagittae), and is likely to be subject to similar ecological pressures. In this shared environment, H. nicaraguensis has to compete for territory space with Amphilophus species, and also defend its juveniles against them ([25], personal observations). Compared to the two Amphilophus species, H. nicaraguensis also has much more pronounced sexual size dimorphism, with males of H. nicaraguensis often reaching the size of small adult A. sagittae (≥15 cm total length), while female H. nicaraguensis are considerably smaller (typically below 10 cm) [25, 29]. Male and female H. nicaraguensis also have slightly different colour markings, with the latter possessing a prominent dark lateral stripe. By contrast, male and female Amphilophus do not differ in coloration, although males within a pair are slightly larger [25, 27, 29] and tend to have longer fin filaments than females.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Amphilophus sagittae territory holders attacking a model of A. astorquii with non-breeding body markings. The male is closer to the camera, with the female only partially visible behind him

Fig. 2
figure 2

The total rate of aggression by A. sagittae and H. nicaraguensis territory holders (a specimen of each of the two species is pictured at the bottom of the graph) towards non-breeding (boxes with vertical stripes) versus breeding (solid dark boxes) intruder models of A. astorquii (pictured on the top of the graph). The results are given separately for the two sexes of the territory holders. Central horizontal lines within the boxes indicate means, margins of the boxes are for standard errors, and whiskers indicate standard deviations. Sample size for each box: n = 28

Breeding and non-breeding individuals posing different threat levels to territory holders allowed us to test whether territory holders adjust their aggression depending on the presumed threat status of heterospecific intruders. We hypothesised that body colour patterns related to differences in breeding status in A. astorquii intruders would more likely influence territorial aggression in the congeneric and phenotypically similar A. sagittae compared to the dissimilar H. nicaraguensis (Fig. 2). Specifically, if recognition of intruder breeding status (based on coloration) is stronger when the intruder is phenotypically similar (see [2]), we would expect A. sagittae territory holders to make a clearer distinction between A. astorquii intruders differing in breeding status than H. nicaraguensis territory holders. The use of an allopatric intruder, A. astorquii (which is endemic to Lake Apoyo), controlled for the opportunity for stimulus learning. This is relevant because familiarity is known to often affect aggression in general [3032], with opportunities for learning potentially also influencing heterospecific aggression [8, 9]. Similarly, if the sexes differ in their sensitivity towards the type of intruder (e.g. due to different abilities in heterospecific recognition, sensu Ord et al. [5]), we would also expect to see the pattern of responses to the different intruder types to be sex-specific (i.e. we would expect to find evidence of a sex × intruder type interaction).

Methods

This field-based study was conducted using SCUBA in Lake Xiloá, Nicaragua (12°12.8′ N; 86°19.0′ W) between December 2013 and January 2014, during the breeding season of our two focal species, Amphilophus sagittae and Hypsophrys nicaraguensis ([23, 24], personal observations). In particular, we investigated the effect of phenotypic similarity on the level of territorial aggression towards non-breeders vs. breeders, while controlling for the opportunity for stimulus learning by using an allopatric intruder, A. astorquii. In addition, we explicitly controlled for any effects that might otherwise be caused by the behaviour of the stimulus, by using intruder models (or ‘dummies’). Such models have been successfully used to study behaviour in a range of fish species (reviewed by Rowland [33]), including Amphilophus [18, 19, 34] and other cichlids [3537]. Instead of the stylised fish models that have been used in past studies [3335], we opted for more realistic-looking models based on photographs of wild-caught fish following the methods of Lehtonen [18]. Specifically, we glued a waterproof, photographic colour print of the lateral side of a live or freshly euthanized specimen (sex unknown or not noted) onto each lateral side of an elliptical floating plate with a thickness of 6 mm. All our models were l6 cm long and attached to a sinker with a thin, transparent fishing line, so that they floated in a natural position approximately 15 cm above the lake bottom during the trials ([18, 19, 38]; Fig. 1). Models of this size were easy to handle under water and represented an overlap in the size ranges of adult male and female A. sagittae [19] as well as male H. nicaraguensis.

In our model presentation (see below), the breeder and non-breeder models (based on 24 different A. astorquii individuals) were paired. In 12 model pairs, the non-breeder model was, as explained above, based on a photo of a non-breeding individual, which was then also used to generate a ‘breeding’ counterpart by manipulating the image in Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems, Inc., San Jose, CA, USA) until it resembled a fish with the uniform (i.e. non-barred) markings of a breeding individual. Each ‘breeder’ counterpart had therefore exactly the same shape and posture as compared to its ‘non-breeder’ model pair, with colour markings being the only difference between the two models. For the rest (n = 12), in turn, the non-breeder counterpart model was made by manipulating the image of a breeding individual (in terms of shading and contrast) so that the horizontal bars became visible and the fish resembled a non-breeder (Figs. 1 and 2). Hence, in total we had 24 fixed model pairs, with each model pair made using a photograph from a different individual. These 24 models pairs were presented to 28 A. sagittae and 28 H. nicaraguensis territory-holding pairs defending small fry in a habitat characterised by pebbles lying on a finer substratum of sand and organic material. As a result, four model pairs were used twice for both focal species, while the rest of the models were used only once per species. This design was accounted for in the statistical analyses (see below).

Each trial started by placing an A. astorquii model approximately 40 cm from the centre of the focal territory, which is at, or slightly below, an average distance territory holders swim when deterring territorial intruders [22, 27, 39]. We counted the total number of aggressive responses (either slow movement toward the model with flared gills and fins in a threat display or a rapid advance sometimes followed by a bite [18, 19]) by both male and female territory owners towards the model for 5 min (giving the total aggression rate sensu [19, 27]). We then removed the model from sight for a predetermined rest period of 5 min, after which we repeated the above with the alternative model (i.e. a breeder model if the initial model was a non-breeder and vice versa). The model type (breeder versus non-breeder) presented first was randomised. After the trial, the territory was marked with a numbered piece of ceramic tile to avoid assessing the same territory more than once.

Statistical analyses

To assess the effects of the species (A. sagittae versus H. nicaraguensis) and sex (male versus female) of the focal territory holder, as well as the type of model intruder (breeder versus non-breeder), we applied a generalized mixed model using the ‘glmmpql’ function of the packages ‘nlme’ and ‘MASS’ with a negative binomial error distribution appropriate for over-dispersed count data [40]. To account for the non-independence of the actions of a territory-holding male and female, as well as any effects related to the use of stimulus model pairs in more than one replicate, ‘breeding pair/territory ID’ and ‘model pair ID’ were added as random effects (as per [41]). We then proceeded with stepwise refits of the model, each time without its least significant, highest order interaction term, using p = 0.05 as the cut-off point. We used R 3.2.2 software (R Development Core Team) for all analyses.

Results

When we applied a generalized mixed model to assess the effects of the species and sex of the focal territory holders and the ‘breeder/non-breeder’ status of the model intruder on the rate of aggression, we found a significant interaction between focal species and sex (t219 = 6.38, p < 0.001): male H. nicaraguensis exhibited a higher rate of aggression than females, whereas there was no pronounced sex difference in A. sagittae (Fig. 2). The effect of intruder status (i.e. breeder versus non-breeder colour markings) was not significant (t219 = 1.05, p = 0.29) (Fig. 2). We also considered the possibility that our results might have been affected by the artificial manipulation of our models (i.e. image manipulation in Photoshop). We assessed this by reanalysing the data comparing only the aggressive responses towards A. astorquii models that exhibited natural (i.e. non-manipulated) breeding versus non-breeding colour patterns. The results, however, remained qualitatively the same: there was an interaction between sex and species (t107 = 4.20, p < 0.001), whereas the status of the model (i.e. breeder versus non-breeder) did not have a significant effect (t107 = 0.818, p = 0.41).

Discussion

We found that neither H. nicaraguensis nor A. sagittae territory holders reacted differently to breeder vs. non-breeder model intruders of A. astorquii. In other words, contrary to our expectation, neither of the two focal species adjusted their aggression to the allopatric heterospecific signal. Earlier studies using both manipulated [19] and natural [22] stimuli have nevertheless strongly indicated that Amphilophus cichlids do react differently to sympatric breeders and non-breeders, with coloration (of model intruders) being a sufficient cue for aggression level adjustments in both of our focal species [18, 19, 38]. Below we discuss why we did not find adjustment of aggression towards the different models of A. astorquii intruders in the current study.

First, we consider the possibility that one or both focal species had the capacity to correctly distinguish between breeder and non-breeder (model) intruders but chose not to modify their aggression because the signal was not clear or sufficiently relevant to induce a response. In the case of H. nicaraguensis, it remains possible, for example, that differences in the motivations of intruders of the more distantly related Amphilophus, as displayed by the breeding and non-breeding colour patterns, are not relevant enough for territory holders to significantly adjust their aggressive behaviour. However, this possibility is less likely to explain why the closely related and phenotypically similar A. sagittae territory holders also did not respond differently to the two breeder types, even though they do direct more aggression towards models of breeders compared to non-breeders of the sympatric A. xiloaensis [19]. It is nevertheless feasible that to avoid any costs of misplaced aggression more generally, both species may have evolved, or territory holders may have learned, not to modify their territorial aggression when the stimulus cues do not match well enough with the specific signals that are displayed by conspecifics or phenotypically similar species with which they are sympatric. Next, we consider proximate mechanisms that could have resulted in the lack of response to the allopatric breeding status signal. In other words, we consider the possibility that the territory holders might simply have not succeeded in making the distinction between breeders and non-breeders when these were allopatric.

At the proximate level, it is feasible that mere differences in markings and colour brightness between allopatric breeders and non-breeders, without any supporting behavioural differences, may have given too subtle a cue for the territory holders to adjust their aggression. In other words, because the ability to distinguish between breeders and non-breeders [19, 22] has, by default, evolved in interaction with species sharing the same environment (i.e. sympatric species), the territory holders may not be able to recognise the equivalent cues when signalled by allopatric species. This possibility supports the hypothesis and empirical observations that interactions with non-native competitors or predators can result in inappropriate behavioural responses [27, 4244]. For instance, the results reported in the current study are consistent with an earlier study investigating the response of Amphilophus zaliosus parents towards an introduced predator, the bigmouth sleeper (Gobiomorus dormitor) in Lake Apoyo [27]. That study showed that fry-guarding parents allowed the non-native predator to venture much more closely to their fry before reacting to them compared to the distance that native fish predators were allowed to approach. Hence, signals used to recognise competitors or predators may result in inappropriate behavioural responses when individuals are exposed to novel or unfamiliar signals, which can have negative fitness consequences for the receiver and/or benefit the novel (invasive) species [42, 45]. In this respect, if our results are due to a failure of the focal Lake Xiloá residents in recognising the breeding status signal of allopatric A. astorquii intruders, we do not currently know whether the observed response would have been an overreaction to breeders or an underreaction towards non-breeders. In the case of an actual invader, a likely consequence of the former would be increased energy expenditure, whereas the latter could result in increased rates of predation on eggs and juveniles (see [27, 44]).

Finally, we consider the possibility that our models simply did not accurately represent differences between A. astorquii breeders vs. non-breeders. In this respect, we prepared our models by adjusting shading and contrast of one model in each pair to mimic the patterns of the opposite breeding status. However, we do not believe that this artificial manipulation of colour patterns per se explains the results. This is because even when we analysed the reactions towards A. astorquii models with natural breeding and non-breeding coloration, we still found no difference in response towards the two colour types. Furthermore, our models were thinner than actual fish, and we therefore cannot rule out the possibility that territory holders may have perceived the models as individuals in poor body condition. If this was the case, territory holders might have regarded the models as a lower threat compared to living intruders in good condition. However, it is important to point out that earlier studies have demonstrated significant aggression adjustments to colour differences in similar intruder models (i.e. with a thickness of 6 mm) of sympatric species [18, 19, 38].

Due to the argument of Ord et al. [5] that sex differences in the opportunity or ability to learn relevant cues may induce differences in heterospecific recognition between males and females, we also assessed differences between the sexes in their reactions towards breeder versus non-breeder models, when deliberately controlling for learning opportunities by using an allopatric stimulus. Our results do not provide evidence for sex differences in recognition (or relevance) of the allopatric signal. We did nevertheless find an overall sex difference in aggressiveness in H. nicaraguensis but not in A. sagittae. This result is likely to reflect a general difference in sex roles between these two species. In particular, it seems that in H. nicaraguensis, more so than in A. sagittae, males and females have evolved divergent roles in territory defence. Specifically, we found that H. nicaraguensis males were far more aggressive compared to females, whereas aggressive responses were much more evenly distributed between the sexes in A. sagittae. We note that the size of our intruder models (total length: 16 cm) relative to self may have been perceived more similarly between the sexes in A. sagittae as compared to H. nicaraguensis, given that males are only slightly larger than females in the former (typical male standard length: 13–18 cm, typical female standard length: 10–15 cm) but much larger than females in the latter (typical male standard length: 8–12 cm, typical female standard length 4–8 cm) [25, 29].

Conclusion

We found that although the two focal species share the same breeding habitat and are likely to be subject to similar ecological pressures in the shared environment, they nevertheless exhibited different sex roles in breeding territory defence. This means that different species have evolved divergent approaches for successful parental care. However, we did not find evidence for differentiation between sexes in the pattern of aggression adjustment in either species. Interestingly, we found that when the stimulus was allopatric, aggressive defence of the breeding territory was not adjusted towards stimuli with contrasting breeding status coloration, independent of the phenotypic similarity between the territory holders and intruders. This is in contrast to earlier studies that used similar methodology but with sympatric (rather than allopatric) intruder stimuli. When considered together with these earlier findings, the current results underscore the importance of considering familiarity and coevolution in heterospecific competitor recognition.

Ethics

The study was approved by MARENA (Ministerio del Ambiente y los Recursos Naturales, Nicaragua: permit no. 013-102013) and is compliant with all relevant laws for the ethical treatment of animals in scientific research.

Availability of supporting data

Our data have been uploaded to Dryad: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.79p2b.

References

  1. Grether GF, Losin N, Anderson CN, Okamoto K. The role of interspecific interference competition in character displacement and the evolution of competitor recognition. Biol Rev. 2009;84:617–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Peiman KS, Robinson BW. Ecology and evolution of resource-related heterospecific aggression. Q Rev Biol. 2010;85:133–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Grether GF. The neuroecology of competitor recognition. Integr Comp Biol. 2011;51:807–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Grether GF, Anderson CN, Drury JP, Kirschel ANG, Losin N, Okamoto K, et al. The evolutionary consequences of interspecific aggression. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2013;1289:48–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ord TJ, King L, Young AR. Contrasting theory with the empirical data of species recognition. Evolution. 2011;65:2572–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sih A. Understanding variation in behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change: a conceptual overview. Anim Behav. 2013;85:1077–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Drury JP, Okamoto KW, Anderson CN, Grether GF. Reproductive interference explains persistence of aggression between species. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20142256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Dijkstra PD, Seehausen O, Fraterman RE, Groothuis TGG. Learned aggression biases in males of Lake Victoria cichlid fish. Anim Behav. 2008;76:649–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Svensson EI, Eroukhmanoff F, Kristina K, Runemark A, Brodin A. A role for learning in population divergence of mate preferences. Evolution. 2010;64:3101–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol Evol. 2003;18:41–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Houston AI, Székely T, McNamara JM. Conflict between parents over care. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20:33–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Arnold AP. Sex chromosomes and brain gender. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:701–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Jacobs GH, Rowe MP. Evolution of vertebrate colour vision. Clin Exp Optom. 2004;87:206–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Barlow GW. Contrasts in social behavior between Central American cichlid fishes and coral-reef surgeon fishes. Am Zool. 1974;14:9–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Seehausen O, Mayhew PJ, Van Alphen JJM. Evolution of colour patterns in East African cichlid fish. J Evol Biol. 1999;12:514–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Elmer KR, Kusche H, Lehtonen TK, Meyer A. Local variation and parallel evolution: morphological and genetic diversity across a species complex of neotropical crater lake cichlid fishes. Philos T Roy Soc B. 2010;365:1763–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Geiger MF, McCrary JK, Schliewen UK. Not a simple case–a first comprehensive phylogenetic hypothesis for the Midas cichlid complex in Nicaragua (Teleostei: Cichlidae: Amphilophus). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2010;56:1011–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lehtonen TK. Colour biases in territorial aggression in a Neotropical cichlid fish. Oecologia. 2014;175:85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lehtonen TK, Sowersby W, Gagnon K, Wong BBM. Cichlid fish use coloration as a cue to assess the threat status of heterospecific intruders. Am Nat. 2015;186:547–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Dijkstra PD, Seehausen O, Pierotti MER, Groothuis TGG. Male–male competition and speciation: aggression bias towards differently coloured rivals varies between stages of speciation in a Lake Victoria cichlid species complex. J Evol Biol. 2007;20:496–502.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pauers MJ, Kapfer JM, Fendos CE, Berg CS. Aggressive biases towards similarly coloured males in Lake Malawi cichlid fishes. Biol Lett. 2008;4:156–9.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lehtonen TK, McCrary JK, Meyer A. Territorial aggression can be sensitive to the status of heterospecific intruders. Behav Process. 2010;84:598–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lehtonen TK. Convict cichlids benefit from close proximity to another species of cichlid fish. Biol Lett. 2008;4:610–2.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lehtonen TK, Wong BBM, Lindström K, Meyer A. Species divergence and seasonal succession in rates of mate desertion in closely related Neotropical cichlid fishes. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:607–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. McKaye KR. Competition for breeding sites between the cichlid fishes of Lake Jiloá, Nicaragua. Ecology. 1977;58:291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Barlow GW. The cichlid fishes: nature’s grand experiment in evolution. Cambridge: Perseus Publishing; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Lehtonen TK, McCrary JK, Meyer A. Introduced predator elicits deficient brood defence behaviour in a crater lake fish. PLoS One. 2012;7, e30064.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Rogers W. Parental investment and division of labor in the Midas cichlid (Cichlasoma citrinellum). Ethology. 1988;79:126–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. McKaye KR. Mate choice and size assortative pairing by the cichlid fishes of Lake Jiloá, Nicaragua. J Fish Biol. 1986;29:s135–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kareem AM, Barnard CJ. The importance of kinship and familiarity in social interactions between mice. Anim Behav. 1982;30:594–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Temeles EJ. The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’? Anim Behav. 1994;47:339–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Backwell PRY, Jennions MD. Coalition among male fiddler crabs. Nature. 2004;430:417.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Rowland WJ. Studying visual cues in fish behavior: a review of ethological techniques. Env Biol Fish. 1999;56:285–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Barlow GW, Siri P. Polychromatic Midas cichlids respond to dummy opponents: color, contrast and context. Behaviour. 1994;130:77–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Beeching SC. Colour pattern and inhibition of aggression in the cichlid fish Astronotus ocellatus. J Fish Biol. 1995;47:50–8.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Beeching SC, Gross SH, Bretz HS, Hariatis E. Sexual dichromatism in convict cichlids: the ethological significance of female ventral coloration. Anim Behav. 1998;56:1021–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Ochi H, Awata S. Resembling the juvenile colour of host cichlid facilitates access of the guest cichlid to host territory. Behaviour. 2009;146:741–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Lehtonen TK, Sowersby W, Wong BBM. Heterospecific aggression towards a rarer colour morph. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20151551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Lehtonen TK, Wong BBM, Svensson PA, Meyer A. Adjustment of brood care behaviour in the absence of a mate in two species of Nicaraguan crater lake cichlids. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:613–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Zuur AF, Hilbe J, Ieno EN. A Beginner’s guide to GLM and GLMM with R: a frequentist and Bayesian perspective for ecologists. Newburgh (UK): Highland Statistics; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Statistics and computing series. New York (NY): Springer; 2000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  42. Cox JG, Lima SL. Naiveté and an aquatic–terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of introduced predators. Trends Ecol Evol. 2006;21:674–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, et al. Predator–prey naïveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos. 2010;119:610–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Wong BBM, Candolin U. Behavioral responses to changing environments. Behav Ecol. 2015;26:665–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Salo P, Korpimäki E, Banks PB, Nordström M, Dickman CR. Alien predators are more dangerous than native predators to prey populations. Proc R Soc B. 2007;274:1237–43.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Marta Barluenga, Lasse and Pirjo Lehtonen, Ken McKaye, Mark McKaye, Eric van den Berghe, and Jeffrey McCrary for logistic support. Funding was provided by the Department of Biology at the University of Turku, the Holsworth Wildlife Endowment Fund, and the Linnean Society of New South Wales.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Topi K. Lehtonen.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

TKL conceived the study, and had the leading role in designing the study, performing the experiment, and writing the manuscript. BBMW and WS provided ideas for the study design and, together with KG, contributed to preparations for the experiment. KG participated in gathering the data, and together with TKL, analysed the data. All authors played a role in preparing the manuscript and approved the final version of it.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lehtonen, T.K., Gagnon, K., Sowersby, W. et al. Allopatry, competitor recognition and heterospecific aggression in crater lake cichlids. BMC Evol Biol 16, 3 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0569-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0569-9

Keywords