Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Is sperm morphology functionally related to sperm swimming ability? A case study in a wild passerine bird with male hierarchies

Abstract

Background

Sexual selection continues after copulation via either sperm competition or cryptic female choice, and favors sperm traits that maximize sperm competitiveness. Both sperm swimming velocity and longevity are important determinants of the outcome of sperm competition. Theoretically, sperm morphology can influence sperm velocity at least in three different non-exclusive ways: (i) longer sperm may generate more propelling thrust, (ii) bigger midpieces may produce more energy, and/or (iii) larger flagella or mid-pieces relative to the head size may compensate for the drag forces around the head. A growing number of studies have investigated the relationship of sperm morphology with sperm performance, which remains equivocal at both the inter- and intra-specific levels. Here, we used House Sparrows to test the functional relationship between sperm morphology with sperm velocity and longevity. Based on a previous study showing that sperm swimming ability covaries with social rank, we predicted that —if a functional relationship exists—1) sperm morphology should differ across social ranks, and 2) correlations between sperm morphology and sperm velocity and/or sperm longevity should be constant across social ranks.

Results

We found no differences in sperm morphology across social ranks. Moreover, we found that sperm morphology may be correlated with sperm velocity, but such relationship varied across social ranks. This result contradicts the hypothesis of a functional relationship between sperm morphology and sperm performance. Finally, after experimentally manipulating social ranks, we observed that relationships between sperm morphology and sperm velocity and/or sperm longevity disappeared or changed direction.

Conclusions

We suggest that in species with internal fertilization, while sperm morphology is likely constrained by the morphology of the female sperm storage organs, selection may act upon physiological traits that enhance sperm performance. Hence, these two selection forces could decouple sperm performance from sperm morphology.

Background

When females copulate with more than one male, strong selection acts upon traits that maximize sperm competitiveness [1,2,3]. Postcopulatory sexual selection can be the result of either cryptic female choice or competition between sperm of different males, known as sperm competition, and both are hypothesized to drive the evolution of sperm morphology [1, 4, 5]. For instance, in many taxa sperm morphology has coevolved with that of the female reproductive tract reviewed in [6], and in birds it has been observed that sperm storage tubule length correlates with sperm length [7,8,9,10]. Moreover, across taxa it has also been observed that sperm morphology correlates with the intensity of sperm competition [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. However, we understand less well how differences in sperm morphological traits translate into better competitive fertilization success [19].

Sperm velocity has been found to be a major determinant of sperm fertilization success in invertebrates [20], fish [21, 22], mammals [23, 24], and birds [25, 26]. Among the various non-exclusive ways in which sperm design can increase sperm velocity, it has been proposed that (1) longer sperm can benefit from a higher flagellar thrust [13], (2) longer midpieces can both produce larger amounts of energy to fuel the flagellar thrust [27] and –in the case of bird sperm- stabilize the flagellum during the screw-like motion [28], (3) and/or longer flagella or midpieces relative to the head size can compensate for the drag forces around the head by providing either more energy (i.e. midpiece) or propelling power (i.e. flagellum) [29]. Evidence that longer sperm have higher swimming velocities exists in various taxa [13, 28, 30, 31]. Similarly, mid-piece length has been found to positively correlate with swimming speed [27, 32]. Sperm morphology has also been hypothesized to affect other traits than sperm velocity. For example, longer mid-pieces relative to the size of the spermatozoon may prevent the energy reserves from being exhausted, thus sustaining protracted viability. In support of this hypothesis, spermatozoa with longer mid-pieces relative to head size have been found to live longer [33].

When access to fertile females is biased towards males of a given phenotype or engaged in different social roles (e.g. dominant vs. subordinate, territorial vs. sneaker), theory predicts that as males have lower access to females they should increase expenditure into post-copulatory traits [3, 34, 35]. Evidence validating the predictions of these models has been found in insects [36], fish [37, 38], birds [39], and mammals [40, 41]. Consequently, if sperm morphology functionally correlates with sperm performance, we predict that in species where male dominance covaries with sperm performance, sperm morphology should also covary with male dominance. However, predictions on the direction of the differences in sperm morphology across a dominance hierarchy are difficult to formulate, given that the functional relation between sperm morphology and function is as yet unclear.

House sparrows (Passer domesticus) are socially monogamous passerine birds exhibiting significant levels of sperm competition and extrapair paternity (12–18%) resulting from female promiscuity and forced copulations [42,43,44,45,46,47]. It is worth noting that although older house sparrow males seem to monopolize extra-pair paternities [43], Møller [48] has shown that more dominant males obtain more extra-pair copulations. Moreover, we have shown that male House Sparrows occupying different social ranks produce sperm performing differently [49]. In the current study, we used data from this previous study [49] and added morphological measurements of spermatozoa to test whether sperm performance is functionally related to sperm morphology. We predicted that, if sperm morphology explains variation in sperm performance, 1) sperm morphology should explain the differences we found in sperm performance across social ranks and should thus differ across social ranks, and 2) the direction of any correlations between sperm morphology and sperm velocity and/or sperm longevity should remain qualitatively the same across all social ranks.

Results

Sperm morphology and social status

Before manipulating the social status, we were unable to obtain a sperm sample from one male, thus our results are based on ejaculates from 59 males. We did not find any differences in total sperm length or sperm design in relation to male social rank (Table 1). However, total sperm length was positively correlated with male body mass (Table 1). After manipulating the social status, the newly stablished hierarchical ranks did not explain variation in total sperm length and sperm design (Table 2). However, for both total sperm length and sperm design a significant interaction arose between social rank and body mass (Table 2). Specifically, while for dominant males both total sperm length and sperm design decrease with body mass, for all the subordinate males both traits increase with body mass (Additional file 1: Figure S1).

Table 1 Summary from the linear mixed models exploring the role of social rank explaining variation in sperm design and total sperm length before manipulating the social status
Table 2 Summary from the linear mixed models exploring the role of social rank explaining variation in sperm design and total sperm length after manipulating the social status

Relationships between sperm design and sperm function

Before manipulating the social status, we found that males occupying different social status differed in their sperm velocity and proportion of motile sperm (Tables 3 and 4). Further, we found that the relation between sperm velocity and sperm design across time (e.g. ability to maintain their speed) differed across social ranks (Table 3; Fig. 1). A similar result was found for the relation between sperm velocity and total sperm length for different social ranks (Table 4; Additional file 1: Figure S2). We also found that the rate at which the proportion of motile sperm decreased with time differed between social ranks (Tables 3 and 4). Finally, our results revealed that the relation between sperm design and the initial proportion of motile sperm also differed between social ranks (Table 3; Fig. 2A).

Table 3 Relation between sperm functional traits and sperm design
Table 4 Relation between sperm functional traits and total sperm length
Fig. 1
figure 1

Relation between the decay in sperm velocity (VCL) through time and sperm design across social ranks (a-d) before manipulating the social environment. The surfaces were obtained from predicted values extracted from linear mixed models. Larger PC1 values indicate larger flagella and mid-pieces relative to head size, and they are centered by social rank

Fig. 2
figure 2

Relation between sperm design and the initial proportion of motile sperm across social ranks both (a) before and (b) after the social status manipulation. For more details, refer to Table 3

After manipulating the social status, we found no differences across ranks for the relationship between sperm velocity and sperm design (Table 3; Fig. 3) or total sperm length (Table 4; Additional file 1: Figure S3). Further, the relation between sperm design and the initial proportion of motile sperm disappeared after the social status manipulation (Table 3; Fig. 2B)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Relation between the decay in sperm velocity (VCL) across time and sperm design across social ranks (a-d) after manipulating the social environment. The surfaces were obtained from predicted values extracted from linear mixed models. Larger PC values indicate larger flagella and mid-pieces relative to head size, and they are centered by social rank

Discussion

In this study, we aimed at testing whether sperm performance is functionally related to sperm morphology in a passerine bird, the house sparrow. Based on results from a previous study showing that sperm performance varies according to male social status, we predicted that, if sperm morphology explains variation in sperm performance, 1) sperm morphology should differ across social ranks, and 2) the direction of the correlations between sperm morphology and sperm velocity and/or sperm longevity should remain similar across all social ranks. We found no differences in sperm morphology across social ranks (Table 1). Moreover, we found that the correlations between sperm velocity and sperm design (Fig. 1) or total sperm length (Additional file 1: Figure S2) depended on male social rank. Specifically, we found that among dominant and subordinate-1 male sperm design (the size of the flagellum and the midpiece relative to the size of the sperm head) was unrelated to the rate at which sperm velocity decays through time. In contrast, subordinate-2 males producing spermatozoa with shorter flagellum and mid-piece relative to the head (Fig. 1) suffered from a steeper decay in sperm velocity through time. Finally, the pattern was opposite in subordinate-3 males. Similar patterns were observed for total sperm length (Additional file 1: Figure S2). Therefore, our results do not support the hypothesis that sperm morphology is functionally related to sperm swimming ability, and sperm morphology cannot explain the covariation between sperm motility and social status observed previously [49].

It has been previously hypothesized that larger flagella and mid-pieces help spermatozoa generate more thrust while maintaining high energetic demands that are required to sustain a larger flagellum, which in turn could lead to a faster swimming speed [29]. Our results provide, at best, mixed support for this hypothesis (see Fig. 1). Moreover, after experimentally manipulating the social environment we did not observe changes in either sperm design or total sperm length that matched the new social ranks, and most of the previously observed correlations between sperm design and swimming performance disappeared or changed direction (Fig. 2B, 3, and Additional file 1: Figure S3). These results further challenge the idea of a functional relationship between sperm morphology and sperm performance (e.g. velocity, longevity, motility), and rather suggest that many of the observed correlations do not reflect causality between the two traits.

In House Sparrows, a previous study found that sperm with bigger heads relative to the flagellum swim at a lower speed [33]. Yet, another study found the opposite relationship [50]. Here, we found that the relationships between sperm design and sperm performance varied depending on male social status. Thus, it could be argued that sampling bias towards bolder, more dominant vs. shier, more subordinate males might explain the discrepancies between the two previous studies. After we experimentally manipulated the social environment, many of the observed correlations between sperm morphology and performance disappeared. However, our previous data on house sparrows suggest that males are able to adjust their sperm quality to match their new social ranks [49], and the results of the present study indicate that such adjustments do not require any changes in sperm morphology.

Previous comparative studies have found differences in sperm morphology across species characterized by different risks of sperm competition [15, 17, 51, 52]. In house sparrows, large between-male variation in sperm morphology has been reported [33]. Yet, neither sperm performance nor social dominance explain why males differ so much in sperm morphology (this study). Plasticity in sperm morphology has been also reported in passerine birds, but whether such changes in morphology would result in changes in sperm performance is not clear. Specifically, it has been found that an experimental manipulation of the perceived male-male competition levels leads to changes in sperm morphology in Gouldian Finches [53]. Here, we did not find any changes in sperm morphology after experimentally modifying males’ social status, which is expected given the lack of correlation between sperm performance and morphology.

A possible explanation for why sperm performance covaries with social rank [49] and sperm morphology does not (the current study) is that sperm morphology is constrained by the morphology of the female sperm storage organs. Between bird species, a positive correlation between avian sperm size and the length of the sperm storage tubules (SSTs) suggests that the female reproductive tract can exert selection on sperm size [54]. Thus within species, stabilizing selection towards an optimal sperm length that matches SSTs could constrain plasticity in sperm morphology, while post-copulatory sexual selection (either through sperm competition or female cryptic choice) may still favor other mechanisms that enhance ejaculate fertilizing ability to match the risk of sperm competition faced by a male. Consequently, we suggest that these two different selection forces may eventually decoupled sperm morphology from sperm performance in the case of house sparrows.

It has been found that increasing levels of sperm competition might select for larger sperm mitochondrial loads in primates [11], while evidence in other taxa has shown that sperm ATP content is positively correlated with sperm motility [55,56,57]. For instance, males at different social ranks may differ in the initial ATP stores in their sperm cells e.g. ATP; [58, 59], and thus males might be able to adjust their sperm motility without changing their sperm morphology. Yet, mitochondrial energy production can release by-products that can cause oxidative damage [60], which disrupts sperm membranes [61]. Indeed, it has been observed that sperm swimming ability is negatively correlated with the levels of membrane oxidation [62, 63]. Consequently, rather than modifying their sperm morphology, males may be selected to modify their sperm performance via differential resource allocation to protect sperm from ROS, a hypothesis supported by results showing that the levels of oxidative damage to sperm vary across social ranks [49]. Alternatively, seminal fluid composition has been shown to have a positive impact on sperm performance [64], and indeed it has been observed in fowls that dominant males can strategically inseminate females with faster swimming sperm due to the seminal fluids used for the ejaculate [65].

Conclusion

To conclude, we found that, in house sparrows, sperm morphology does not reflect sperm performance, yet it is worth noting that our assays were done in an artificial medium that does not necessarily reflect the conditions experience by sperm within the female reproductive tract. Nonetheless, we believe that while sperm morphology might be constrained by the length of the female sperm storage tubules, selection may still act upon sperm physiology to enhance traits associated with sperm performance. We suggest that, as a consequence of such different selective pressures, sperm design may be functionally decoupled from sperm performance. Finally, we encourage researchers focusing on the relationships between sperm morphology and sperm performance to include both information about the males’ reproductive opportunities (e.g. risk of sperm competition) and sperm physiology.

Methods

Individuals and sampling

We trapped 60 male and 60 female House Sparrows in western Switzerland in April 2014. Before transferring birds to 15 mixed outdoor aviaries at the Hasli Ethological Station (University of Bern, Switzerland), we measured their body mass and tarsus length. Additionally, birds were scored for their badge size (1–5 following the diagram in Fig. 1 from [66]) and assigned to their aviaries based both on their badge size score and their body mass to ensure that all aviaries would contain approximately the same distribution of small-badged, medium-badge and large-badged males and thus would exhibit approximately the same distribution in relative badge size (and potentially competitive ability), and to ensure that average body mass did not significantly differ across aviaries. Males in each aviary were banded with either a red, white, yellow, or orange color ring and a unique ID metal ring. After four weeks of acclimatization, females were transferred to a separate aviary, and we took a sperm sample from each male. We then collected a second sperm sample the day after, and a third sperm sample after 48 h from the last sample. This procedure ensured that any differences in sperm characteristics would be intrinsic differences in quality rather than differences due to depletion [40, 67] or fresh sperm effects [68,69,70]. To maintain sampling time within reasonable limits per day, males were divided in three sampling batches of five aviaries, and batches were processed with a 5-day gap.

Females were then reintroduced to the aviaries and males were shuffled between aviaries according to their initial social rank, as to optimize the number of movements upwards and downwards in the hierarchies (For details on the procedure, see Additional file 2: Table S1). Males were given three weeks to settle into their new social ranks, to ensure that all males went through at least one spermatogenic cycle [71]. Finally, a sperm sampling session was done as described above.

Behavioral observations and assessment of social ranks

During the acclimatization period, we performed a total of 13 h of video recordings on each aviary to establish the hierarchical position of each male, and recordings were done once a day for one hour. Each aviary had a tower feeder mounted on a plastic plate that collected all the spilt seeds through a plastic mesh, making food only accessible at the two feeder openings. Feeders were removed each morning for 90 min, and a GoPro camera was introduced with the feeders. From the videos, we counted the number of antagonistic interactions (fights and chasing) between males, and used those data to calculate a David’s score [72]. After manipulating the social status, we established the social ranks from 10 h of video from each aviary, following the same procedures.

Sperm morphology and sperm swimming performance

Samples were obtained by gently massaging males’ cloacae [73], and ejaculates were collected in 5 μl glass capillaries. Right after collection, 0.25 μL of ejaculate were diluted in 40 μL of preheated Dulbecco Modified Eagle Medium at 40 °C, and then a 20 μm chamber (Leja Products B.V., The Netherlands) was loaded with the diluted ejaculate. Immediately after loading the chamber, a video was done using a Toshiba CMOS HD camera (TOSHIBA Corporation, Japan) camera mounted on a light microscope at 100× magnification and phase contrast 3 annular ring, while keeping a constant temperature of 40 °C with a heating plate mounted on the microscope (Minitube HT200 W, MINITÜB GmbH, Germany). A small droplet from the ejaculate was smeared with 10% formalin on a glass slide for sperm morphology assessments. From each sample, we took photos of ten sperm cells, with coiled midpiece and unbroken heads, using the Nikon ACT-1 v2.70 software (Nikon Corporation, Japan) for a Nikon Digital Eclipse DXM1200 camera (Nikon Corporation, Japan) mounted on a Leica DM R microscope (Leica Microsystems GmbH, Germany) at 400× magnification and phase contrast 2.

From the videos, we estimated the curvilinear velocity (VCL) and the proportion of motile sperm using a computer automated sperm analyser plug-in [74] for ImageJ [75]. Sperm cells having a VSL < 5 μm/s, a VCL < 15 μm/s, or a VAP < 10 μm/s were considered as either moved by drift or immotile. Each video was sampled at 0, 15, 30, 60 and 90 s after the beginning of the recording, thus allowing us to estimate sperm longevity and sperm ability to maintain initial speed. From each photo, we measured the straight head, midpiece, flagellum, and total length, and S.C. made all the morphological measurements to avoid errors due to observer bias in the measurements. Each photo was independently measured twice blind to the previous measurements, and the average between the two measurements was used for further analyses. All samples were processed blind to male identity and social rank.

Statistical analyses

For each male, we calculated the average in sperm morphology from the 10 spermatozoa, and then we used these mean measures in further analyses. To estimate sperm design, we performed a principal component analysis using head, mid-piece, and flagellum lengths, and we then extracted the first component as a measurement of sperm design (Rotations PC1: head length = 0.008, mid-piece length = 0.59, flagellum length = 0.81; Proportion of variance explained by PC1 = 0.74). Thus, males with higher scores along the PC1 produced spermatozoa with longer mid-pieces and flagella, relative to sperm head size. To test whether males at different social ranks differed in sperm morphology or design, we ran linear mixed models using total sperm length or sperm design as response variable and social rank as a predictor variable. Additionally, body weight and tarsus length, both centered on rank, were used as covariates as well as their interactions with social rank, while the aviary and the collection batch were entered as random intercepts.

To test whether total sperm length or sperm design would explain variation in sperm swimming performance, we performed linear mixed models using the proportion of motile sperm and the curvilinear velocity (VCL) as response variables. The predictor variables were the social rank, total sperm length or sperm design centered on rank, the time after the video recording started, and the interactions between them, while body weight and tarsus length were used as covariates. The models included the aviary and male identity as random intercepts, as well as the interaction between male identity and time to model random slopes.

After experimentally modifying the social environments, we tested whether the patterns observed before manipulating the social status were maintained. Thus, we ran similar models but using the data collected after the social status manipulation, as well as the newly stablished social ranks.

In all models, proportion of motile sperm was logit transformed to match normality. All the models were done using a restricted maximum likelihood method for parameter estimation, and Kenward-Roger approximation of fixed effects degrees of freedom. We did not apply model selection to avoid inflating the type I error probability [76]. All the analyses were performed using R v 3.3.2.

References

  1. Birkhead TR, Pizzari T. Postcopulatory sexual selection. Nat Rev Genet. 2002;3(4):262–73.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Parker GA. Sperm competition and the evolution of ejaculates: towards a theory base. In: Birkhead TR, Møller AP, editors. Sperm competition and sexual selection. London, UK: Academic Press; 1998. p. 3–54.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  3. Parker GA, Pizzari T. Sperm competition and ejaculate economics. Biol Rev. 2010;85(4):897–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Fitzpatrick JL, Lüpold S. Sexual selection and the evolution of sperm quality. Mol Hum Reprod. 2014;20(12):1180–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Snook RR. Sperm in competition: not playing by the numbers. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20(1):46–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Lüpold S, Pitnick S. Sperm form and function: what do we know about the role of sexual selection? Reproduction. 2018;155(5):R229–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Dybas LK, Dybas HS. Coadaptation and taxonomic differentiation of sperm and spermathecae in featherwing beetles. Evolution. 1981;35(1):168–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Miller GT, Pitnick S. Sperm-female coevolution in drosophila. Science. 2002;298(5596):1230–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Presgraves DC, Baker RH, Wilkinson GS. Coevolution of sperm and female reproductive tract morphology in stalk–eyed flies. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;266(1423):1041–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Higginson DM, Miller KB, Segraves KA, Pitnick S. Female reproductive tract form drives the evolution of complex sperm morphology. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109(12):4538–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Anderson MJ, Dixson AF. Sperm competition: motility and the midpiece in primates. Nature. 2002;416(6880):496–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Anderson MJ, Nyholt J, Dixson AF. Sperm competition and the evolution of sperm midpiece volume in mammals. J Zool. 2005;267(2):135–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gomendio M, Roldan ERS. Sperm competition influences sperm size in mammals. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1991;243(1308):181–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Tourmente M, Gomendio M, Roldan ERS: Sperm competition and the evolution of sperm design in mammals. BMC Evol Biol 2011, 11(1):1–10.

  15. Balshine S, Leach BJ, Neat F, Werner NY, Montgomerie R. Sperm size of African cichlids in relation to sperm competition. Behav Ecol. 2001;12(6):726–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gage MJG. Associations between body size, mating pattern, testis size and sperm lengths across butterflies. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1994;258(1353):247–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Immler S, Birkhead TR. Sperm competition and sperm midpiece size: no consistent pattern in passerine birds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;274(1609):561–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Morrow EH, Gage MJG. The evolution of sperm length in moths. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000;267(1440):307–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Hemmings N, Bennison C, Birkhead TR. Intra-ejaculate sperm selection in female zebra finches. Biol Lett. 2016;12(6).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kupriyanova E, Havenhand JN. Variation in sperm swimming behaviour and its effect on fertilization success in the serpulid polychaete Galeolaria caespitosa. Invertebrate Reproduction & Development. 2002;41(1–3):21–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Gage MJG, Macfarlane CP, Yeates S, Ward RG, Searle JB, Parker GA. Spermatozoal traits and sperm competition in Atlantic salmon: relative sperm velocity is the primary determinant of fertilization success. Curr Biol. 2004;14(1):44–7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gasparini C, Simmons LW, Beveridge M, Evans JP. Sperm swimming velocity predicts competitive fertilization success in the green swordtail Xiphophorus helleri. PLoS One. 2010;5(8):e12146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Malo AF, Garde JJ, Soler AJ, García AJ, Gomendio M, Roldan ERS. Male fertility in natural populations of red deer is determined by sperm velocity and the proportion of normal spermatozoa. Biol Reprod. 2005;72(4):822–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Holt C, Holt WV, Moore HDM, Reed HCB, Curnock RM. Objectively measured boar spermmotility parameters correlate with the outcomes of on-farm inseminations: results of two fertility trials. J Androl. 1997;18(3):312–23.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Froman DP, Feltmann AJ, Rhoads ML, Kirby JD. Sperm mobility: a primary determinant of fertility in the domestic fowl (Gallus domesticus). Biol Reprod. 1999;61(2):400–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Birkhead TR, Martinez JG, Burke T, Froman DP. Sperm mobility determines the outcome of sperm competition in the domestic fowl. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;266(1430):1759–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Cardullo RA, Baltz JM. Metabolic regulation in mammalian sperm: mitochondrial volume determines sperm length and flagellar beat frequency. Cell Motil Cytoskeleton. 1991;19(3):180–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Lüpold S, Calhim S, Immler S, Birkhead TR. Sperm morphology and sperm velocity in passerine birds. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276(1659):1175–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Humphries S, Evans J, Simmons L. Sperm competition: linking form to function. BMC Evol Biol. 2008;8(1):319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Mossman J, Slate J, Humphries S, Birkhead T. Sperm morphology and velocity are genetically codetermined in the zebra finch. Evolution. 2009;63(10):2730–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Fitzpatrick JL, Garcia-Gonzalez F, Evans JP. Linking sperm length and velocity: the importance of intramale variation. Biol Lett. 2010;6(6):797–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Firman RC, Simmons LW. Sperm midpiece length predicts sperm swimming velocity in house mice. Biol Lett. 2010;6(4):513–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Helfenstein F, Podevin M, Richner H. Sperm morphology, swimming velocity, and longevity in the house sparrow Passer domesticus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64(4):557–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Tazzyman Samuel J, Pizzari T, Seymour Robert M, Pomiankowski A. The evolution of continuous variation in ejaculate expenditure strategy. Am Nat. 2009;174(3):E71–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Parker GA, Lessells CM, Simmons LW. Sperm competition games: a general model for precopulatory male-male competition. Evolution. 2013;67(1):95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Thomas ML, Simmons LW. Male dominance influences pheromone expression, ejaculate quality, and fertilization success in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behav Ecol. 2009;20(5):1118–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Rudolfsen G, Figenschou L, Folstad I, Tveiten H, Figenschou M. Rapid adjustments of sperm characteristics in relation to social status. Proc R Soc B. 2006;273(1584):325–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Evans JP. Quantitative genetic evidence that males trade attractiveness for ejaculate quality in guppies. Proc R Soc B. 2010;277(1697):3195–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Cornwallis Charlie K, Birkhead Tim R. Changes in sperm quality and numbers in response to experimental manipulation of male social status and female attractiveness. Am Nat. 2007;170(5):758–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Pemberton JM, Wilson K. Dominant rams lose out by sperm depletion. Nature. 2001;409(6821):681–2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Lemaître JF, Ramm SA, Hurst JL, Stockley P. Sperm competition roles and ejaculate investment in a promiscuous mammal. J Evol Biol. 2012;25(6):1216–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Edly-Wright C, Schwagmeyer PL, Parker PG, Mock DW. Genetic similarity of mates, offspring health and extrapair fertilization in house sparrows. Anim Behav. 2007;73(2):367–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hsu YH, Schroeder J, Winney I, Burke T, Nakagawa S. Are extra-pair males different from cuckolded males? A case study and a meta-analytic examination. Mol Ecol. 2015;24(7):1558–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Birkhead TR, Veiga JP, Fletcher F. Sperm competition and unhatched eggs in the house sparrow. J Avian Biol. 1995;26(4):343–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Møller AP. House sparrow, Passer domesticus, communal displays. Anim Behav. 1987;35(1):203–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Møller AP, Birkhead TR. The evolution of plumage brightness in birds is related to extrapair paternity. Evolution. 1994;48(4):1089–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Wetton JH, Parkin DT. An association between fertility and cuckoldry in the house sparrow, Passer domesticus. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1991;245(1314):227–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Møller AP. Frequency of female copulations with multiple males and sexual selection. Am Nat. 1992;139(5):1089–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Rojas Mora A, Meniri M, Gning O, Glauser G, Vallat A, Helfenstein F. Antioxidant allocation modulates sperm quality across changing social environments. PLoS One. 2017;12(5):e0176385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Cramer ERA, Laskemoen T, Stensrud E, Rowe M, Haas F, Lifjeld JT, Sætre G-P, Johnsen A. Morphology-function relationships and repeatability in the sperm of Passer sparrows. J Morphol. 2015;276(4):370–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. LaMunyon CW, Samuel W. Evolution of sperm size in nematodes: sperm competition favours larger sperm. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;266(1416):263–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Lüpold S, Linz G, Birkhead T. Sperm design and variation in the New World blackbirds (Icteridae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009;63(6):899–909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Immler S, Pryke SR, Birkhead TR, Griffith SC. Pronounced within-individual plasticity in sperm morphometry across social environments. Evolution. 2010;64(6):1634–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Briskie JV, Montgomerie R. Sperm size and sperm competition in birds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1992;247(1319):89–95.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Christen R, Gatti J-L, Billard R. Trout sperm motility. The transient movement of trout sperm is related to changes in the concentration of ATP following the activation of the flagellar movement. Eur J Biochem. 1987;166(3):667–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Lahnsteiner F, Berger B, Weismann T, Patzner RA. Motility of spermatozoa ofAlburnus alburnus (Cyprinidae) and its relationship to seminal plasma composition and sperm metabolism. Fish Physiol Biochem. 1996;15(2):167–79.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Froman DP, Feltmann AJ. Sperm mobility: a quantitative trait of the domestic fowl (Gallus domesticus). Biol Reprod. 1998;58(2):379–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Burness G, Casselman SJ, Schulte-Hostedde AI, Moyes CD, Montgomerie R. Sperm swimming speed and energetics vary with sperm competition risk in bluegill (Lepomis macrochirus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;56(1):65–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Burness G, Moyes CD, Montgomerie R. Motility, ATP levels and metabolic enzyme activity of sperm from bluegill (Lepomis macrochirus). Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2005;140(1):11–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hulbert AJ, Pamplona R, Buffenstein R, Buttemer WA. Life and death: metabolic rate, membrane composition, and life span of animals. Physiol Rev. 2007;87(4):1175–213.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Aitken RJ, Baker MA. Oxidative stress and male reproductive biology. Reprod Fertil Dev. 2004;16(5):581–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. Helfenstein F, Losdat S, Møller AP, Blount JD, Richner H. Sperm of colourful males are better protected against oxidative stress. Ecol Lett. 2010;13(2):213–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Losdat S, Richner H, Blount JD, Helfenstein F. Immune activation reduces sperm quality in the great tit. PLoS One. 2011;6(7):e22221.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Poiani A. Complexity of seminal fluid: a review. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60(3):289–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Cornwallis CK, O'Connor EA. Sperm: seminal fluid interactions and the adjustment of sperm quality in relation to female attractiveness. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276(1672):3467–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Møller AP. Variation in badge size in male house sparrows Passer domesticus: evidence for status signalling. Anim Behav. 1987;35(6):1637–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Wesseling C, Fasel NJ, Richner H, Helfenstein F. Modification of sperm quality after sexual abstinence in the Seba's short-tailed bat (Carollia perspicillata). Journal of Experimental Biology Accepted.

  68. Siva-Jothy MT. The young sperm gambit. Ecol Lett. 2000;3(3):172–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Pizzari T, Dean R, Pacey A, Moore H, Bonsall MB. The evolutionary ecology of pre- and post-meiotic sperm senescence. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008;23(3):131–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Firman RC, Young FJ, Rowe DC, Duong HT, Gasparini C. Sexual rest and post-meiotic sperm ageing in house mice. J Evol Biol. 2015;28(7):1373–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Jones RC, Lin M. Spermatogenesis in birds. Oxf Rev Reprod Biol. 1993;15:233–64.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Gammell MP, de Vries H, Jennings DJ, Carlin CM, Hayden TJ. David’s score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.’s index. Anim Behav. 2003;66(3):601–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Wolfson A. The cloacal protuberance: a means for determining breeding condition in live male passerines. Bird-Banding. 1952;23(4):159–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Wilson-Leedy JG, Ingermann RL. Development of a novel CASA system based on open source software for characterization of zebrafish sperm motility parameters. Theriogenology. 2007;67(3):661–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9(7):671–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Whittingham MJ, Stephens PA, Bradbury RB, Freckleton RP. Why do we still use stepwise modelling in ecology and behaviour? J Anim Ecol. 2006;75(5):1182–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the Laboratory of Behavioural Ecology at the university of Bern for granting us access to their aviaries. We also thank the farmers who allowed us to catch birds in their properties.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation n° PP00P3_139011 to FH.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset used in the current study is available from the corresponding author on request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

FH & ARM designed the study. ARM, MM & SC conducted the experiment, and collected the data. ARM analysed the sperm videos and SC measured the sperm. ARM & FH performed statistical analyses and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alfonso Rojas Mora.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted under the licenses n° BE41/12 and WTH/g-525/14 for animal experimentation by the Veterinary Office in the Canton Bern, Switzerland.

Consent of publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Figure S1. Relation between sperm morphological design and male body mass centered by social ranks. Lines represent a linear regression, whereas colors indicate dominant (D) and subordinate- 1 to 3 (S1–3) males. Figure S2. Relation between the decay in sperm velocity (VCL) through time and sperm total length across social ranks before manipulating the social environment. The surfaces were obtained from predicted values extracted from linear mixed models. Values are centered by social rank. Figure S3. Relation between the decay in sperm velocity (VCL) through time and sperm total length across social ranks after manipulating the social environment. The surfaces were obtained from predicted values extracted from linear mixed models. Values are centered by social rank. (DOCX 2323 kb)

Additional file 2:

Table S1. Male social status manipulation. The manipulation was performed equally for each experimental block of 5 aviaries, and the net change reflects the number of social ranks that individual were expected to gain or lose (the net change adds up to zero). (XLSX 9 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rojas Mora, A., Meniri, M., Ciprietti, S. et al. Is sperm morphology functionally related to sperm swimming ability? A case study in a wild passerine bird with male hierarchies. BMC Evol Biol 18, 142 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-018-1260-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-018-1260-8

Keywords